NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 52 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Alicia K. Jones; Shalini Gautam; Jonathan Redshaw – Child Development, 2025
Counterfactual emotions such as regret may aid future decision-making by encouraging people to focus on controllable features of personal past events. However, it remains unclear when children begin to preferentially focus on controllable features of such events. Across two studies, Australian 4-9-year-olds (N = 336, 168 females; data collected…
Descriptors: Childrens Attitudes, Psychological Patterns, Decision Making, Emotional Response
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schleihauf, Hanna; Herrmann, Esther; Fischer, Julia; Engelmann, Jan M. – Child Development, 2022
We investigate how the ability to respond appropriately to reasons provided in discourse develops in young children. In Study 1 (N = 58, Germany, 26 girls), 4- and 5-, but not 3-year-old children, differentiated good from bad reasons. In Study 2 (N = 131, Germany, 64 girls), 4- and 5-year-old children considered both the strength of evidence for…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Young Children, Beliefs, Thinking Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Leshin, Rachel A.; Leslie, Sarah-Jane; Rhodes, Marjorie – Child Development, 2021
A problematic way to think about social categories is to essentialize them--to treat particular differences between people as marking fundamentally distinct social kinds. From where do these beliefs arise? Language that expresses generic claims about categories elicits some aspects of essentialism, but the scope of these effects remains unclear.…
Descriptors: Language Attitudes, Beliefs, Childrens Attitudes, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
González, Meliza; Loose, Tianna; Liz, Maite; Pérez, Mónica; Rodríguez-Vinçon, Juan I.; Tomás-Llerena, Clementina; Vásquez-Echeverría, Alejandro – Child Development, 2022
The COVID-19 context has created the most severe disruption to education systems in recent history. Its impact on child development was estimated comparing two cohorts of 4- to 6-year-old Uruguayan children: control (n = 34,355, 48.87% girls) and COVID cohort (n = 30,158, 48.95% girls) assessed between 2018 and 2020 in three waves, by a routinely…
Descriptors: School Readiness, COVID-19, Pandemics, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rakoczy, Hannes; Bergfeld, Delia; Schwarz, Ina; Fizke, Ella – Child Development, 2015
Existing evidence suggests that children, when they first pass standard theory-of-mind tasks, still fail to understand the essential aspectuality of beliefs and other propositional attitudes: such attitudes refer to objects only under specific aspects. Oedipus, for example, believes Yocaste (his mother) is beautiful, but this does not imply that…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Beliefs, Young Children, Educational Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tenenbaum, Harriet R.; Ruck, Martin D. – Child Development, 2012
This study examined British young people's understanding of the rights of asylum-seeking young people. Two hundred sixty participants (11-24 years) were read vignettes involving asylum-seeking young people's religious and nonreligious self-determination and nurturance rights. Religious rights were more likely to be endorsed than nonreligious…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Young Adults, Adolescents, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Campbell, John D. – Child Development, 1975
Examines themes evident in children's and mother's definitions of illness to determine how views of illness develop. Subjects were 264 children (ages 6 to 12-11) and their mothers. Two issues were considered: (a) patterned similarity in illness definitions, and (b) developmental changes in illness concepts. (ED)
Descriptors: Attitude Change, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Samuels, Mark C.; McDonald, John – Child Development, 2002
Two experiments compared 10-year-olds' and adults' ability to choose positive and negative diagnostic tests over positive and negative nondiagnostic tests. Findings indicated that both age groups were more likely to prefer positive diagnostic tests over positive nondiagnostic tests, although only adults showed a significant preference for negative…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Attitudes, Childhood Attitudes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, Edward A. – Child Development, 1997
Used stories involving self-deception, lying, and misleading appearances to examine 4- to 9-year olds' understanding of the relations between false belief, evidence, and epistemic responsibility. Found that younger children who understood false belief understood simpler types of deception but that understanding self-deceivers' epistemic…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Deception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hickling, Anne K.; Gelman, Susan A. – Child Development, 1995
Examined young children's understanding of seed origins and growth preconditions and the stages of plant growth. Found that, by 4.5 years, children realized that natural causal mechanisms underlie plant growth and appreciated the relationship of seeds to plants. Results suggest that preschoolers hold theory-like understandings of plants similar to…
Descriptors: Botany, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Gardening
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Barrett, Justin L.; Richert, Rebekah A.; Driesenga, Amanda – Child Development, 2001
Three experiments examined assumption that children attribute human properties to nonhuman agents. Two- to 8-year-olds participated in false-belief tests concerning human and various nonhuman agents, including animals and God, and in a modified perspective-taking task including nonhuman agents. Results suggested that children do not consistently…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Childhood Attitudes, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Siegler, Robert S.; Svetina, Matija – Child Development, 2006
Learning of class inclusion by 5-year-olds in response to empirical and logical explanations of an adult's answers was examined. Contrary to the view that young children possess an empirical bias, 5-year-olds learned more, and continued learning for longer, when given logical explanations of correct answers than when given empirical explanations.…
Descriptors: Young Children, Social Bias, Logical Thinking, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Powell, Louisa Feldman – Child Development, 1974
Low-birth-weight infants, who received extra stimulation showed improved development through six months of age. (ST)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Infants, Mother Attitudes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lyon, Thomas D.; Flavell, John H. – Child Development, 1994
Three studies examined young children's understanding that, if one "remembers" or "forgets," one must have known something previously. The majority of four-year olds, but not three-year olds, understood that, when two characters currently knew something, the one with prior knowledge remembered and that, when neither character…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mitchell, P.; Robinson, E. J. – Child Development, 1994
Three experiments tested four- to seven-year olds' ability to understand and reconcile message-desire discrepant stories. The findings suggest that young children can refrain from a performative response and, as a consequence, attend to literal meaning under some conditions when evaluating utterances. (MDM)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Childhood Attitudes, Cognitive Development, Evaluation
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4