NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pronovost, Megan A.; Scott, Rose M. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2021
Adults use social-group membership to make inductive inferences about the properties of novel individuals, and this tendency is well established by the preschool years. Recent evidence suggests that infants attend to features associated with social groups and use social-group membership to interpret an agents' actions. The current study sought to…
Descriptors: Social Influences, Inferences, Logical Thinking, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Liu, In-mao; Chou, Ting-hsi – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2015
How likely is the glass to break, given that it is heated? The present study asks questions such as this with or without the premise "if the glass is heated, it breaks." A reduced problem (question without premise) measures the statistical dependency (conditional probability) of an event to occur, given that another has occurred. Such…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Cognitive Development, Probability, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Butler, Lucas P.; Markman, Ellen M. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2012
In making causal inferences, children must both identify a causal problem and selectively attend to meaningful evidence. Four experiments demonstrate that verbally framing an event ("Which animals make Lion laugh?") helps 4-year-olds extract evidence from a complex scene to make accurate causal inferences. Whereas framing was unnecessary when…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Inferences, Evidence, Logical Thinking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rhodes, Marjorie; Gelman, Susan A.; Brickman, Daniel – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2008
Determining whether a sample provides a good basis for broader generalizations is a basic challenge of inductive reasoning. Adults apply a diversity-based strategy to this challenge, expecting diverse samples to be a better basis for generalization than homogeneous samples. For example, adults expect that a property shared by two diverse mammals…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Age Differences, Grade 1, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lawson, Christopher A.; Kalish, Charles W. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2006
Young children tend to expect that 2 members of the same category will share properties, yet they frequently deny that an individual's properties will remain stable across time and context. Two experiments, involving 72 four- to five-year-olds, 72 seven- to eight-year-olds, and 76 undergraduates, explored the factors that lead children to…
Descriptors: Inferences, Logical Thinking, Young Children, Undergraduate Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Deneault, Joane; Ricard, Marcelle – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2006
This study investigated the development of the understanding of class inclusion in children age 5, 7, and 9 years, whose performance on a qualitative class-inference task assessing their appreciation of the transitive and asymmetrical nature of inclusive relations within the animal domain was compared with their ability to make quantitative…
Descriptors: Children, Inferences, Cognitive Development, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
DeLoache, Judy S.; Sharon, Tanya – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2005
Surface similarity generally promotes reasoning by analogy and physical similarity has been shown to have a powerful positive effect on very young children's use of a scale model as a source of information about another space. The research reported here investigated 2 1/2-year-old children's performance in an object retrieval task when asked to…
Descriptors: Transfer of Training, Inferences, Cognitive Development, Logical Thinking