NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 9 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hsieh, Po-Jang; Colas, Jaron T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The contents of working memory (WM) have predominantly been viewed as necessarily conscious. However, recent findings suggest otherwise. Here we investigate whether visual WM can represent subliminal stimuli, such that the positions of an invisible moving object can be extrapolated or learned about in terms of their task-relevant predictive power.…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Patching, Geoffrey R.; Englund, Mats P.; Hellstrom, Ake – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Despite the importance of both response probability and response time for testing models of choice, there is a dearth of chronometric studies examining systematic asymmetries that occur over time- and space-orders in the method of paired comparisons. In this study, systematic asymmetries in discriminating the magnitude of paired visual stimuli are…
Descriptors: Computation, Visual Stimuli, Probability, Reaction Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lindsen, Job P.; de Jong, Ritske – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Lien, Ruthruff, Remington, & Johnston (2005) reported residual switch cost differences between stimulus-response (S-R) pairs and proposed the partial-mapping preparation (PMP) hypothesis, which states that advance preparation will typically be limited to a subset of S-R pairs because of structural capacity limitations, to account for these…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Visual Discrimination, Reaction Time, Hypothesis Testing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jordan, J. Scott; Hunsinger, Matthew – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2008
When participants control the horizontal movements of a stimulus and indicate its vanishing point after it unexpectedly vanishes, the perceived vanishing point is displaced beyond the actual vanishing point, and the size of the displacement is directly related to the action-effect anticipation one has to generate to successfully control the…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Experiments, Spatial Ability, Visual Discrimination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kravitz, Dwight Jacob; Behrmann, Marlene – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2008
Although object-based attention enhances perceptual processing of information appearing within the boundaries of a selected object, little is known about the consequences for information in the object's surround. The authors show that distance from an attended object's center of mass determines reaction time (RT) to targets in the surround. Of 2…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Dimensional Preference, Information Processing, Proximity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Santangelo, Valerio; Spence, Charles – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
We compared the ability of auditory, visual, and audiovisual (bimodal) exogenous cues to capture visuo-spatial attention under conditions of no load versus high perceptual load. Participants had to discriminate the elevation (up vs. down) of visual targets preceded by either unimodal or bimodal cues under conditions of high perceptual load (in…
Descriptors: Cues, Attention Control, Attention, Visual Discrimination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Whitney, David – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
Perceived position depends on many factors, including motion present in a visual scene. Convincing evidence shows that high-level motion perception-which is driven by top-down processes such as attentional tracking or inferred motion-can influence the perceived position of an object. Is high-level motion sufficient to influence perceived position,…
Descriptors: Motion, Perception, Experiments, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Fockert, Jan; Davidoff, Jules; Fagot, Joel; Parron, Carole; Goldstein, Julie – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
The Ebbinghaus (Titchener) illusion was examined in a remote culture (Himba) with no words for geometric shapes. The illusion was experienced less strongly by Himba compared with English participants, leading to more accurate size contrast judgments in the Himba. The study included two conditions of inducing stimuli. The illusion was weaker when…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Visual Discrimination, Misconceptions, Cross Cultural Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vroomen, Jean; Keetels, Mirjam – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
A sound presented in temporal proximity to a light can alter the perceived temporal occurrence of that light (temporal ventriloquism). The authors explored whether spatial discordance between the sound and light affects this phenomenon. Participants made temporal order judgments about which of 2 lights appeared first, while they heard sounds…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Sensory Integration, Acoustics, Proximity