NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 25 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sho Ohigashi; Shuhei Takagi; Yusuke Moriguchi – European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2024
Emotion labels can be helpful for creating emotion categories. Russell and Widen (2002) demonstrated the label superiority effect; that is, emotion labels produce a more precise categorization of emotional faces than the corresponding emotional faces. The current study aimed to test the label superiority effect on emotional voices and examined…
Descriptors: Emotional Intelligence, Nonverbal Learning, Pictorial Stimuli, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sophie Wacker; Claudia M. Roebers – Metacognition and Learning, 2024
When young children evaluate their confidence, their monitoring is often overoptimistic, that is, inaccurate. The present study investigated a potential underlying mechanism for kindergarteners' and second graders' overconfidence within a paired associates learning paradigm. We implemented a pre-monitoring phase motivating children to…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Decision Making, Comparative Analysis, Student Motivation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roebers, Claudia M.; Kälin, Sonja; Aeschlimann, Eva A. – Metacognition and Learning, 2020
In the past decade, research increasingly uncovers emerging metacognitive skills in young children by using child-friendly, creative, and non-verbal measures of metacognition. In the present study, the now often used non-verbal "opt-out" paradigm and classical verbal metacognitive judgments (judgments-of-learning and confidence…
Descriptors: Verbal Communication, Nonverbal Communication, Metacognition, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Olivia Rush; Krystal L. Werfel; Emily Lund – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2023
Purpose: This study compares responses of children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) who use spoken language with responses of children who have typical hearing on a repeated word association task to evaluate lexical-semantic organization. Method: This study included 109 participants in early kindergarten or who had completed first grade. The…
Descriptors: Grade 1, Kindergarten, Young Children, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Barrón-Martínez, Julia B.; Arias-Trejo, Natalia; Salvador-Cruz, Judith – International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 2022
From the second year of life, children with typical development (TD) demonstrate the ability to form word-word relations. However, this ability has received little attention in children with Down syndrome (DS). We investigated their ability to establish associative relationships between words that tend to occur in the same context. Two groups of…
Descriptors: Down Syndrome, Language Acquisition, Language Skills, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Altarelli, Irene; Dehaene-Lambertz, Ghislaine; Bavelier, Daphne – Developmental Science, 2020
Audio-visual associative learning -- at least when linguistic stimuli are employed -- is known to rely on core linguistic skills such as phonological awareness. Here we ask whether this would also be the case in a task that does not manipulate linguistic information. Another question of interest is whether executive skills, often found to support…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Associative Learning, Visual Learning, Language Skills
Aravind, Athulya; de Villiers, Jill; Pace, Amy; Valentine, Hannah; Golinkoff, Roberta; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Iglesias, Aquiles; Wilson, Mary Sweig – Grantee Submission, 2018
Do children learn a new word by tracking co-occurrences between words and referents across multiple instances ("cross-situational learning" models), or is word-learning a "one-track" process, where learners maintain a single hypothesis about the possible referent, which may be verified or falsified in future occurrences…
Descriptors: Young Children, Vocabulary Development, Memory, Retention (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Blything, Ryan P.; Ambridge, Ben; Lieven, Elena V. M. – Cognitive Science, 2018
This study adjudicates between two opposing accounts of morphological productivity, using English past-tense as its test case. The single-route model (e.g., Bybee & Moder, 1983) posits that both regular and irregular past-tense forms are generated by analogy across stored exemplars in associative memory. In contrast, the dual-route model…
Descriptors: English, Grammar, Morphemes, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Sharma Mittal, Ruhi; Nagar, Seema; Sharma, Mourvi; Dwivedi, Utkarsh; Dey, Prasenjit; Kokku, Ravi – International Educational Data Mining Society, 2018
As education gets increasingly digitized, and intelligent tutoring systems gain commercial prominence, scalable assessment generation mechanisms become a critical requirement for enabling increased learning outcomes. Assessments provide a way to measure learners' level of understanding and difficulty, and personalize their learning. There have…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Language Tests, Semantics, Associative Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wismer Fries, Alison B.; Pollak, Seth D. – Developmental Science, 2017
Children who experience early caregiving neglect are very likely to have problems developing and maintaining relationships and regulating their social behavior. One of the earliest manifestations of this problem is reflected in indiscriminate behavior, a phenomenon where young children do not show normative wariness of strangers or use familiar…
Descriptors: Child Neglect, Social Development, Emotional Development, Interpersonal Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Namy, Laura L. – Language Learning and Development, 2012
This paper evaluates the proposal that general associative mechanisms underlie the earliest stages of word learning but that these same general mechanisms, operating over language input, enable children to identify domain-specific cues that ultimately help to constrain word learning, rendering children more sophisticated language users. As a…
Descriptors: Intellectual Disciplines, Vocabulary Development, Cues, Linguistic Input
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Cornejo, Felipe A.; Castillo, Ramon D.; Saavedra, Maria A.; Vogel, Edgar H. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2010
Considerable research has examined the contrasting predictions of configural and elemental associative accounts of learning. One of the simplest methods to distinguish between these approaches is the summation test, in which the associative strength of a novel compound (AB) made of two separately-trained cues (A+ and B+) is examined. The…
Descriptors: Animals, Cues, Classical Conditioning, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sloutsky, Vladimir M.; Fisher, Anna V. – Child Development, 2008
Young children often exhibit flexible behaviors relying on different kinds of information in different situations. This flexibility has been traditionally attributed to conceptual knowledge. Reported research demonstrates that flexibility can be acquired implicitly and it does not require conceptual knowledge. In Experiment 1, 4- to 5-year-olds…
Descriptors: Young Children, Child Behavior, Attention, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Davidson, Denise; Tell, Dina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2005
In two experiments, the use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects was examined in monolingual and bilingual 3- and 6-year-olds. Once an object has a known name, then via principles of mutual exclusivity it is often assumed that a new name given to the object must refer to some part, substance, or other property of the object.…
Descriptors: Monolingualism, Bilingualism, Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Preissler, Melissa Allen; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2005
Young children are readily able to use known labels to constrain hypotheses about the meanings of new words under conditions of referential ambiguity. At issue is the kind of information children use to constrain such hypotheses. According to one theory, children take into account the speaker's intention when solving a referential puzzle. In the…
Descriptors: Inferences, Autism, Language Acquisition, Intention
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2