NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 6 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hughes, Robert W.; Hurlstone, Mark J.; Marsh, John E.; Vachon, Francois; Jones, Dylan M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
The influence of top-down cognitive control on 2 putatively distinct forms of distraction was investigated. Attentional capture by a task-irrelevant auditory deviation (e.g., a female-spoken token following a sequence of male-spoken tokens)--as indexed by its disruption of a visually presented recall task--was abolished when focal-task engagement…
Descriptors: Testing, Selection, Attention, Recall (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tsubomi, Hiroyuki; Ikeda, Takashi; Osaka, Naoyuki – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Perceived brightness is well described by Stevens' power function (S. S. Stevens, 1957, On the psychophysical law, "Psychological Review", Vol. 64, pp. 153-181), with a power exponent of 0.33 (the cubic-root function of luminance). The power exponent actually varies across individuals, yet little is known about neural substrates underlying this…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Diagnostic Tests, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kelly, Ashleigh J.; Dux, Paul E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
To study the temporal dynamics and capacity-limits of attentional selection and encoding, researchers often employ the attentional blink (AB) phenomenon: subjects' impaired ability to report the second of two targets in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream that appear within 200-500 ms of one another. The AB has now been the subject of…
Descriptors: Investigations, Cognitive Processes, Information Processing, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fitousi, Daniel; Wenger, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Variations in perceptual and cognitive demands (load) play a major role in determining the efficiency of selective attention. According to load theory (Lavie, Hirst, Fockert, & Viding, 2004) these factors (a) improve or hamper selectivity by altering the way resources (e.g., processing capacity) are allocated, and (b) tap resources rather than…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Attention, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bruggeman, Hugo; Piuneu, Vadzim S.; Rieser, John J.; Pick, Herbert L., Jr. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
When turning without vision or audition, people tend to perceive their locomotion as a change in heading relative to objects in the remembered surroundings. Such perception of self-rotation depends on sensitivity to information for movement from biomechanical activity of the locomotor system or from inertial activation of the vestibular and…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Psychomotor Skills, Cognitive Processes, Biomechanics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Friedman, Alinda; Polson, Martha Campbell – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1981
A framework for understanding how cerebral specialization of function contributes to the flexibility of human information processing is presented. The left and right hemispheres together form a system of two mutually inaccessible and finite pools of resources which cannot be made available in different amounts at any given time. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Cerebral Dominance, Cognitive Processes, Individual Differences, Literature Reviews