Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Correlation | 10 |
Recall (Psychology) | 6 |
Memory | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Models | 3 |
Prediction | 3 |
Short Term Memory | 3 |
Task Analysis | 3 |
Articulation (Speech) | 2 |
Cognitive Ability | 2 |
Control Groups | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 10 |
Author
Berry, Christopher J. | 1 |
Brewer, Gene A. | 1 |
Caplan, Jeremy B. | 1 |
Corner, Adam | 1 |
Cowan, Nelson | 1 |
Cumming, Nick | 1 |
Delaney, Peter F. | 1 |
Engle, Randall W. | 1 |
Glaholt, Mackenzie G. | 1 |
Hahn, Ulrike | 1 |
Henson, Richard N. A. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lohnas, Lynn J.; Polyn, Sean M.; Kahana, Michael J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
According to contextual-variability theory, experiences encoded at different times tend to be associated with different contextual states. The gradual evolution of context implies that spaced items will be associated with more distinct contextual states, and thus have more unique retrieval cues, than items presented in proximity. Ross and Landauer…
Descriptors: Cues, Recall (Psychology), Probability, Context Effect
Corner, Adam; Hahn, Ulrike; Oaksford, Mike – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Slippery slope arguments (SSAs) have a bad philosophical reputation. They seem, however, to be widely used and frequently accepted in many legal, political, and ethical contexts. Hahn and Oaksford (2007) argued that distinguishing strong and weak SSAs may have a rational basis in Bayesian decision theory. In this paper three experiments…
Descriptors: Probability, Persuasive Discourse, Classification, Correlation
Romero, Kristoffer; Moscovitch, Morris – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Construction of imaginative or fictitious events requires the flexible recombination of stored information into novel representations. How this process is accomplished is not understood fully. To address this problem, older adults (mean age = 74.2; Experiment 1) and younger patients with MTL lesions (mean age = 54.2; Experiment 2), both of whom…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Older Adults, Patients, Memory
Unsworth, Nash; Brewer, Gene A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
The relation between intrusions in several different recall tasks was examined in the current study. Intrusions from these tasks were moderately correlated and formed a unitary intrusion factor. This factor was related to other cognitive ability measures including working memory capacity, judgments of recency, and general source-monitoring…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Ability, Individual Differences
Madan, Christopher R.; Glaholt, Mackenzie G.; Caplan, Jeremy B. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Word properties like imageability and word frequency improve cued recall of verbal paired-associates. We asked whether these enhancements follow simply from prior effects on item-memory, or also strengthen associations between items. Participants studied word pairs varying in imageability or frequency: pairs were "pure" (high-high, low-low) or…
Descriptors: Cues, Holistic Approach, Memory, Word Frequency
Verkoeijen, Peter P. J. L.; Delaney, Peter F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
The "spacing effect" is the commonly observed phenomenon that memory for spaced repetitions is better than memory for massed repetitions. To further investigate the role of rehearsal in spacing effects, three experiments were conducted. With pure lists we found spacing effects in free recall when spacing intervals were relatively long (Experiments…
Descriptors: Intervals, Memory, Recall (Psychology), Correlation
Berry, Christopher J.; Henson, Richard N. A.; Shanks, David R. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
A single-system model of repetition priming and recognition memory is presented, which is conceptually similar to signal-detection theory. Key assumptions of the model are (a) that the same memory source contributes to both priming and recognition performance and (b) that variance of the noise associated with priming measures is greater than…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Models, Attention, Correlation
Page, Mike P. A.; Madge, Alison; Cumming, Nick; Norris, Dennis G. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
In three experiments, we tested the hypothesis that those errors in immediate serial recall (ISR) that are attributable to phonological confusability share a locus with segmental errors in normal speech production. In the first two experiments, speech errors were elicited in the repeated paced reading of six-letter lists. The errors mirrored the…
Descriptors: Phonology, Short Term Memory, Hypothesis Testing, Error Patterns
Jarrold, Christopher; Cowan, Nelson; Hewes, Alexa K.; Riby, Deborah M. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
This study explored the degree of verbal short-term memory deficit among individuals with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome, and the extent to which any such impairment could be accounted for by a relative slowing of rehearsal and output processes. Measures of serial recall and detailed assessments of speeded articulation for short and long…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Down Syndrome, Short Term Memory, Serial Ordering
Oberauer, Klaus; Lange, Elke; Engle, Randall W. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Single-task and dual-task versions of verbal and spatial serial order memory tasks were administered to 120 students tested for working memory capacity with four previously validated measures. In the dual-task versions, similarity between the memory material and the material of the secondary processing task was varied. With verbal material, three…
Descriptors: Memory, Interference (Language), Correlation, Reading Aloud to Others