NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rieger, Martina; Martinez, Fanny; Wenke, Dorit – Cognition, 2011
Using a typing task we investigated whether insufficient imagination of errors and error corrections is related to duration differences between execution and imagination. In Experiment 1 spontaneous error imagination was investigated, whereas in Experiment 2 participants were specifically instructed to imagine errors. Further, in Experiment 2 we…
Descriptors: Keyboarding (Data Entry), Investigations, Office Occupations, Visual Discrimination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sobel, David M. – Cognition, 2009
Two experiments examined whether preschoolers' difficulties on tasks that required relating pretending and knowledge (e.g., Lillard, A. S. (1993a). "Young children's conceptualization of pretense: Action or mental representational state?" "Child Development, 64," 372-386) were due to children's inability to appreciate the causal mechanism behind…
Descriptors: Animals, Preschool Children, Child Development, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rhemtulla, Mijke; Hall, D. Geoffrey – Cognition, 2009
Children's toys provide a rich arena for investigating conceptual flexibility, because they often can be understood as possessing an individual identity at multiple levels of abstraction. For example, many dolls (e.g., Winnie-the-Pooh) and action figures (e.g., Batman) can be construed either as characters from a fictional world or as physical…
Descriptors: Young Children, Play, Child Development, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Kim, Angie L.; Schwalen, Courtney E.; Harris, Paul L. – Cognition, 2009
Based on the testimony of others, children learn about a variety of figures that they never meet. We ask when and how they are able to differentiate between the historical figures that they learn about (e.g., Abraham Lincoln) and fantasy characters (e.g., Harry Potter). Experiment 1 showed that both younger (3- and 4-year-olds) and older children…
Descriptors: Fantasy, History, Young Children, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Friedman, Ori; Leslie, Alan M. – Cognition, 2007
The ability to engage in and recognize pretend play begins around 18 months. A major challenge for theories of pretense is explaining how children are able to engage in pretense, and how they are able to recognize pretense in others. According to one major account, the metarepresentational theory, young children possess both production and…
Descriptors: Play, Young Children, Behavior Theories, Child Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Nichols, Shaun; Stich, Stephen – Cognition, 2000
Presents a theory of pretense in which pretense representations are contained in a separate mental workspace, a Possible World Box, part of the basic architecture of the human mind with several similarities to beliefs. Maintains that pretend play is motivated from a desire to act in a way that fits the description being constructed in the Possible…
Descriptors: Adults, Beliefs, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Richards, Cassandra A.; Sanderson, Jennifer A. – Cognition, 1999
Tested whether 2- to 4-year olds could reason with incongruent syllogisms when encouraged to use their imagination. Randomly assigned 2-, 3-, and 4-year olds to one of four conditions (no cue, word cue, fantasy planet, or imagery) and presented syllogistic reasoning problems with incongruent information. Found that in imagination conditions, 2-…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis, Creative Thinking