Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 3 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 7 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 12 |
Descriptor
Number Concepts | 12 |
Spatial Ability | 12 |
Task Analysis | 12 |
Numeracy | 6 |
Foreign Countries | 5 |
Numbers | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Computation | 3 |
Correlation | 3 |
Cues | 3 |
Coding | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Fias, Wim | 2 |
Gevers, Wim | 2 |
Shaki, Samuel | 2 |
Almor, Amit | 1 |
Bartek, Victoria | 1 |
Boiteau, Timothy W. | 1 |
Branyan, Helen | 1 |
Calleja, Marina | 1 |
Chen, Qi | 1 |
Cooper, Elisheva | 1 |
Damas, Jesús | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Research | 12 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 3 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Israel | 2 |
Belgium | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
Ohio (Dayton) | 1 |
Pennsylvania (Philadelphia) | 1 |
United Kingdom (London) | 1 |
United States | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Woodcock Johnson Tests of… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Nadine Koch; Johannes Lohmann; Martin V. Butz; Hans-Christoph Nuerk – Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2024
Magnitude information, for instance, regarding weight, distance, or velocity, is crucial for planning goal-directed interactions. Accordingly, magnitude information, including numerical magnitude, can affect actions: Responses to small numbers are faster with the left hand than the right and vice versa (hand-based SNARC effect). Previous…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Numbers, Number Concepts, Task Analysis
Branyan, Helen; Cooper, Elisheva; Shaki, Samuel; McCrink, Koleen – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2023
During the preschool years, children are simultaneously undergoing a reshaping of their mental number line and becoming increasingly sensitive to the social norms expressed by those around them. In the current study, 4- and 5-year-old American and Israeli children were given a task in which an experimenter laid out chips with numbers (1-5),…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Memory, Spatial Ability, Number Concepts
Fuhs, Mary Wagner; Tavassolie, Nadia; Wang, Yiqiao; Bartek, Victoria; Sheeks, Natalie A.; Gunderson, Elizabeth A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2021
Young children are sensitive to both numerical and spatial magnitude cues early in development, but many questions remain about how children's attention to magnitudes relates to their early math achievement. In two studies, we tested three hypotheses related to the flexible attention to magnitudes (FAM) account, which suggests that young…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Mathematics Skills, Numeracy, Number Concepts
Boiteau, Timothy W.; Almor, Amit – Cognitive Science, 2017
Previous research has linked the concept of number and other ordinal series to space via a spatially oriented mental number line. In addition, it has been shown that in visual scene recognition and production, speakers of a language with a left-to-right orthography respond faster to and tend to draw images in which the agent of an action is…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Native Language, Verbs, Reading Processes
Noordende, Jaccoline E.; Volman, M(Chiel). J. M.; Leseman, Paul P. M.; Kroesbergen, Evelyn H. – Infant and Child Development, 2017
Previous research suggests that block adding, subtracting and counting direction are early forms of number-space mapping. In this study, an embodiment perspective on these skills was taken. Embodiment theory assumes that cognition emerges through sensory-motor interaction with the environment. In line with this assumption, it was investigated if…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Toys, Handedness, Task Analysis
Thompson, Clarissa A.; Morris, Bradley J.; Sidney, Pooja G. – Grantee Submission, 2017
Do children spontaneously represent spatial-numeric features of a task, even when it does not include printed numbers (Mix et al., 2016)? Sixty first grade students completed a novel spatial estimation task by seeking and finding pages in a 100-page book without printed page numbers. Children were shown pages 1 through 6 and 100, and then were…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Grade 6, Number Concepts, Spatial Ability
Finesilver, Carla – Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal, 2017
The move from additive to multiplicative thinking requires significant change in children's comprehension and manipulation of numerical relationships, involves various conceptual components, and can be a slow, multistage process for some. Unit arrays are a key visuospatial representation for supporting learning, but most research focuses on 2D…
Descriptors: Multiplication, Computation, Numeracy, Number Concepts
Shaki, Samuel; Fischer, Martin H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
A recent cross-cultural comparison (Shaki, Fischer, & Petrusic, 2009) suggested that spatially consistent processing habits for words and numbers are a necessary condition for the spatial representation of numbers (Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes; SNARC effect). Here we reexamine the SNARC in Israelis who read text from right…
Descriptors: Cross Cultural Studies, Number Concepts, Numbers, Spatial Ability
Rodríguez-Santos, José Miguel; Calleja, Marina; García-Orza, Javier; Iza, Mauricio; Damas, Jesús – American Annals of the Deaf, 2014
Deaf Children usually achieve lower scores on numerical tasks than normally hearing peers. Explanations for mathematical disabilities in hearing children are based on quantity representation deficits (Geary, 1994) or on deficits in accessing these representations (Rousselle & Noël, 2008). The present study aimed to verify, by means of symbolic…
Descriptors: Evidence, Deafness, Partial Hearing, Number Concepts
Imbo, Ineke; De Brauwer, Jolien; Fias, Wim; Gevers, Wim – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
In a recent study, Gevers and colleagues (2010, "Journal of Experimental Psychology: General," Vol. 139, pp. 180-190) showed that the SNARC (spatial numerical association of response codes) effect in adults results not only from spatial coding of magnitude (e.g., mental number line hypothesis) but also from verbal coding. Because children are…
Descriptors: Evidence, Experimental Psychology, Number Concepts, Numeracy
Longo, Matthew R.; Lourenco, Stella F. – Brain and Cognition, 2010
Much evidence suggests that common posterior parietal mechanisms underlie the orientation of attention in physical space and along the "mental number line." For example, the small leftward bias ("pseudoneglect") found in paper-and-pencil line bisection is also found when participants "bisect" number pairs, estimating (without calculating) the…
Descriptors: Computation, Number Concepts, Stimuli, Task Analysis
Gevers, Wim; Santens, Seppe; Dhooge, Elisah; Chen, Qi; Van den Bossche, Lisa; Fias, Wim; Verguts, Tom – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
A tight correspondence has been postulated between the representations of number and space. The spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, which reflects the observation that people respond faster with the left-hand side to small numbers and with the right-hand side to large numbers, is regarded as strong evidence for this…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Number Concepts, Correlation, Interaction