Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Attention | 2 |
Interference (Learning) | 2 |
Visual Discrimination | 2 |
Visual Stimuli | 2 |
Coding | 1 |
College Students | 1 |
Color | 1 |
Emotional Response | 1 |
Foreign Countries | 1 |
Pictorial Stimuli | 1 |
Psychological Patterns | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Bluell, Alexandra M. | 1 |
De Houwer, Jan | 1 |
Hughes, Sean | 1 |
Liefooghe, Baptist | 1 |
Montgomery, Derek E. | 1 |
Schmidt, James R. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Belgium | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 2 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Liefooghe, Baptist; Hughes, Sean; Schmidt, James R.; De Houwer, Jan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Automaticity can be established by consistently reinforcing contingencies during practice. During reinforcement learning, however, new relations can also be derived, which were never directly reinforced. For instance, reinforcing the overlapping contingencies A [right arrow] B and A [right arrow] C, can lead to a new relation B-C, which was never…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Visual Stimuli, Interference (Learning), Reaction Time
Bluell, Alexandra M.; Montgomery, Derek E. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2014
The day-night paradigm, where children respond to a pair of pictures with opposite labels for a series of trials, is a widely used measure of interference control. Recent research has shown that a happy-sad variant of the day-night task was significantly more difficult than the standard day-night task. The present research examined whether the…
Descriptors: Pictorial Stimuli, Visual Stimuli, Visual Perception, Visual Discrimination