Descriptor
Source
Author
Ahtee, Maija, Ed. | 1 |
Bybee, Jane | 1 |
Chaiklin, Seth | 1 |
DeRuiter, H. | 1 |
Feldman, Jack M. | 1 |
Fisher, Barbara L. | 1 |
Garofalo, Joe | 1 |
Greenan, James P. | 1 |
Gross, Thomas F. | 1 |
Hein, Michael B. | 1 |
Horton, Jane | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 29 |
Journal Articles | 21 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 9 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Collected Works - Proceedings | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Reports - General | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 35 |
Practitioners | 4 |
Policymakers | 1 |
Teachers | 1 |
Location
Finland | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Wood, Wendy; And Others – 1983
Research on gender differences in group performance suggests that males excel at brainstorming while females excel at human relations and integration. To investigate the relations among gender, interaction style, and task performance, 264 college students (130 female, 134 male) worked in three person same sex groups on a production task which…
Descriptors: College Students, Creative Thinking, Discussion Groups, Group Activities

Chaiklin, Seth – Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal of Artificial Intelligence, Psychology and Language, 1984
This theoretical analysis articulates verbal rule properties and use implications, describes an empirical study examining characteristics of problem solving performance when verbal rules are instructed as a problem solving method, and discusses role of verbal rules in problem solving and implications for learning procedural skills. (MBR)
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Definitions, Learning, Performance
Sanders, Michele; Matsumoto, David – 1984
Recent research has refuted the behaviorist approach by establishing a relationship between emotion and behavior. The data collection procedure, however, has often involved an inferred emotional state from a hypothetical situation. As partial fulfillment of a class requirement, 60 college students were asked to perform two problem solving tasks…
Descriptors: College Students, Emotional Response, Higher Education, Motivation
Stein, Barry S.; And Others – 1983
Research indicates that people do not spontaneously transfer prior clues to solve problems, even though the necessary information is available in memory. To investigate the effects of the symmetry between clue statements and problem statements on problem solving performance, subjects were asked to provide plausible explanations for five…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cues, Generalization, Memory

Klahr, David – Child Development, 1985
Move sequence analysis revealed that, when presented with problems having subgoals difficult to order, 40 preschoolers between 45 and 70 months of age (1) tended to avoid backup; (2) were sensitive to incremental progress toward a goal; and (3) searched moves ahead for a goal. None of several indices of performance were reliably correlated with…
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Cognitive Development, Models, Performance Factors

Mumford, Michael D.; Feldman, Jack M.; Hein, Michael B.; Nagao, Dennis J. – Journal of Creative Behavior, 2001
This study with 432 college students examined how variables influence the relative performance of groups and individuals on creative problem-solving tasks. Having more ideas available (through a priming manipulation) led to better individual performance. Group performance, however, was enhanced by training appropriate to problem content that…
Descriptors: College Students, Creative Thinking, Creativity, Higher Education
Robertson, Ian S. – Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 2004
A sample of 202 students filling in a student evaluation of teaching (SET) questionnaire were asked to complete another questionnaire asking about the specific reasons for awarding a score to the specific SET questionnaire items. The aim was to find out what influenced students' judgements on those items. It was found that students' interpretation…
Descriptors: Student Evaluation of Teacher Performance, Problem Solving, Student Attitudes, Higher Education
Gross, Thomas F. – 1984
Two experiments investigated relationships between state anxiety, memory processes, and children's performance on problem-solving tasks. Participants were second and sixth graders in a private elementary school in Redlands, California. In both experiments, subjects responded to three training and eight test problems presented in the introtact…
Descriptors: Anxiety, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Feedback
Bybee, Jane; Zigler, Edward – American Journal on Mental Retardation, 1992
This study with 56 students (mean age 15 years) with mental retardation and 53 nonretarded students (matched for mental age) found that students with mental retardation were more likely to rely on all kinds of external cues (task-relevant, incidental, or misleading) in problem solving, especially when the preceding task had been difficult.…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Cognitive Processes, Cues, Difficulty Level

Overton, Willis F.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1987
Three experiments explored the development of formal logical reasoning between Grades 4 and 12 and the role of semantic content in the solution of Wason's (1966) selection task problems. Results suggest that formal logical reasoning is not generally present during the fourth or sixth grades and that formal logical competence becomes available in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Deduction, Elementary School Students, Elementary Secondary Education

Weismer, Susan Ellis – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1991
This study, which assessed hypothesis-testing abilities using a discrimination-learning paradigm, found that 16 language-impaired primary-level children solved fewer problems than 16 controls equated on cognitive level, but the 2 groups used similar hypothesis types to solve the problems. Type of verbal feedback (explicit versus nonexplicit) did…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Discrimination Learning, Feedback, Hypothesis Testing

Shore, Bruce M.; And Others – Gifted Child Quarterly, 1994
Reanalysis of the data from a 1984 study on making and breaking problem-solving mental sets with 50 children found that gifted subjects who failed to initially form the set made the most errors of any group and were least likely to recognize their own errors. Results suggest that motivational reasons may underly this inferior performance by some…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Creative Thinking, Elementary Secondary Education

Laughery, K. Ronald – Simulation and Games, 1984
Presents some arguments for new training approaches to teaching people how to play games based on the literature about how humans play games, how they acquire game-playing skills, and game-playing theory. An example of the role-reversal approach to training is presented with some experimental findings regarding its effectiveness. (MBR)
Descriptors: Behavior, Epistemology, Games, Instructional Improvement

Fisher, Barbara L.; And Others – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1996
Comparison of the effects of pretest anxiety on the social and nonsocial problem solving of 45 boys (ages 9 to 11) with learning disabilities (LD) and 45 nondisabled boys found the LD group reported significantly higher pretest anxiety which escalated over the course of the problem-solving session. However, boys with or without LD were equally…
Descriptors: Achievement, Affective Behavior, Intermediate Grades, Learning Disabilities

Robinson, William R.; Niaz, Mansoor – International Journal of Science Education, 1991
The performance of two groups of chemistry students, one taught using the traditional lecture method and the other interactive, in solving stoichiometry problems is described. The interactive instruction appears more effective for students who are less adept at information processing. The interaction apparently does not challenge better students,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Foreign Countries, Interaction, Learning Processes