Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 82 |
Practitioners | 13 |
Teachers | 7 |
Administrators | 2 |
Counselors | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lien, Aaron M.; Ruyle, George; López-Hoffman, Laura – Journal of Extension, 2018
This article introduces Q methodology, an idea-sorting activity that can help Extension improve outreach and education on new and contentious issues. Q methodology is a helpful tool when Extension professionals are confronted with controversial or complex resource management challenges. Through the analysis of a simple card-sorting exercise,…
Descriptors: Q Methodology, Extension Education, Outreach Programs, Controversial Issues (Course Content)
Aldiabat, Khaldoun M.; Le Navenec, Carole-Lynne – Qualitative Report, 2011
Although many researchers have discussed the historical relationship between the Grounded Theory methodology and Symbolic Interactionism, they have not clearly articulated the congruency of their salient concepts and assumptions. The purpose of this paper is to provide a thorough discussion of this congruency. A hypothetical example about smoking…
Descriptors: Grounded Theory, Qualitative Research, Self Concept, Perspective Taking
Ruth, Damian – London Review of Education, 2010
The metaphor of "monoculture on the intellectual landscape" is used to analyse the effects of large-scale research management regimes. The metaphor of "monoculture on the intellectual landscape", derived from Shiva's "monocultures of the mind", is developed and illustrated with respect to gender and ethnicity. The…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Research Administration, Performance Based Assessment, Figurative Language
Mayhew, Matthew J.; Wolniak, Gregory C.; Pascarella, Ernest T. – Research in Higher Education, 2008
We investigated curricular conditions and educational practices that influenced the development of life-long learning orientations among 405 undergraduate students. Results suggest that growth in life-long learning orientations was facilitated by instruction that included opportunities for reflection, active learning, and perspective-taking and…
Descriptors: Undergraduate Students, Learning Strategies, Active Learning, Educational Practices
Barge, J. Kevin; Shockley-Zalabak, Pamela – Journal of Applied Communication Research, 2008
Engaged scholarship represents one way for making our research relevant to organizational practitioners by bridging the gap between theory and practice. Engaged scholarship is viewed as a form of collaborative inquiry between academics and practitioners that leverages their different perspectives to generate useful organizational knowledge. We…
Descriptors: Theory Practice Relationship, Scholarship, Cooperation, Inquiry
Lee, Kyunghwa; Choi, Ikseon – Early Childhood Education Journal, 2008
In this article, we report a research project on web-based case instruction that was developed and implemented to allow prospective early childhood teachers to be exposed to various dilemmas faced by practicing teachers. The goal of this project was to design an instructional tool and method that could help prospective teachers expand their notion…
Descriptors: Classroom Techniques, Focus Groups, Young Children, Data Analysis
Meyer, Ann Jane – 1983
Theorists and researchers have developed abstract divisions in the life span to mark significant stages. To explore the conceptual labels individuals use to make divisions in their life spans, 92 adults, aged 20 to 55 years, marked significant milestones on a lifeline. Participants also completed a demographic questionnaire. Data on the labels…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Aging (Individuals), Developmental Stages

Sodian, Beate; Wimmer, Heinz – Child Development, 1987
Four experiments studied 4- to 6-year-old children's understanding of inferential reasoning as a source of knowledge. To assess understanding that knowledge of relevant premises leads to knowledge of the conclusion, children had to judge the knowledge of another person, who was presented to the child as being aware of two premises. (Author/BN)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Inferences, Metacognition

Taylor, Marjorie – Child Development, 1988
Studies investigated the development of children's ability to differentiate what they see from what they know in the context of conceptual perspective taking. Two developmental levels accounted for children's performance when they were asked about a naive observer's knowledge of the identity of objects. Perspective awareness training improved…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Individual Development, Perspective Taking, Visual Stimuli

Hart, Lynn M.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Child Development, 1984
Children, ages three, five, and seven, were asked to evaluate a series of children's drawings for their own likes and dislikes and for the likes and dislikes they imagined for individuals older and younger than themselves. Results suggest that children as young as three can judge drawings for others differently from the way they judge them for…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Art Products, Egocentrism, Perspective Taking

Keller, M.; Reuss, S. – Human Development, 1984
Outlines how levels of the interpretation of reality and categories of a naive theory of action that constitute these levels are differentiated and coordinated in a specific developmental sequence. Subsumed within this theoretical framework are the distinction between action on physical objects and social interaction and the distinction between…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Conflict, Friendship, Perspective Taking

Lee, Anthony; And Others – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1994
Comparison of the performance of 25 subjects with autism and 25 nonautistic subjects with mental retardation found all subjects able to comprehend the personal pronouns "I,""you," and "me." However, autistic subjects were less likely to employ the pronoun "me" in a visual perspective-taking task and to say "you" to refer to the experimenter.…
Descriptors: Autism, Language Acquisition, Language Skills, Mental Retardation

Radkey, Andrea L.; Enns, James T. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1987
Study separated two components of drawing: perspective taking and graphic skill. Results suggest that failures of young children to draw occlusion have less to do with graphic skill than was previously thought. Results further suggest that young children have a more general difficulty selecting one perspective and maintaining it over time.…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Freehand Drawing, Graphic Arts, Perspective Taking

Blotner, Roberta; Bearison, David J. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1984
Examines the issue of developmental consistencies between and within levels of moral reasoning, altruism, and perspective coordination. The relative contributions of perspective coordination and moral reasoning to altruistic behavior were examined in fifth grade. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Altruism, Children, Context Effect, Helping Relationship

Luckett, T.; Powell, S. D.; Messer, D. J.; Thornton, M. E.; Schulz, J. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2002
This study compared children (n=68) with either autism or general delay on false belief tasks and tasks to test for an understanding of interpretive diversity. Findings partially support the view that tasks of understanding interpretive diversity are more difficult than false belief tasks. Between-group differences in the consistency and quality…
Descriptors: Autism, Beliefs, Children, Cognitive Processes