Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Cues | 2 |
Language Acquisition | 2 |
Predictor Variables | 2 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Attention | 1 |
Autism | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
Computational Linguistics | 1 |
Correlation | 1 |
Form Classes (Languages) | 1 |
Grammar | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 1 |
First Language | 1 |
Author
Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick | 2 |
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy | 2 |
Hennon, Elizabeth A. | 1 |
Lee, Joanne | 1 |
Ma, Weiyi | 1 |
Parish-Morris, Julia | 1 |
Tager-Flusberg, Helen | 1 |
Zhou, Peng | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ma, Weiyi; Zhou, Peng; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Lee, Joanne; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy – First Language, 2019
The syntactic structure of sentences in which a new word appears may provide listeners with cues to that new word's form class. In English, for example, a noun tends to follow a determiner ("a"/"an"/"the"), while a verb precedes the morphological inflection [ing]. The presence of these markers may assist children in…
Descriptors: Syntax, Cues, Mandarin Chinese, Verbs
Parish-Morris, Julia; Hennon, Elizabeth A.; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Tager-Flusberg, Helen – Child Development, 2007
To what extent do children with autism (AD) versus typically developing children (TD) rely on attentional and intentional cues to learn words? Four experiments compared 17 AD children (M age = 5.08 years) with 17 language- and 17 mental-age-matched TD children (M ages = 2.57 and 3.12 years, respectively) on nonverbal enactment and word-learning…
Descriptors: Intention, Cues, Autism, Vocabulary Development