Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Language Processing | 2 |
Memory | 2 |
Auditory Perception | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
Cognitive Psychology | 1 |
Cooperation | 1 |
Experiments | 1 |
Gardening | 1 |
Inhibition | 1 |
Interpersonal Communication | 1 |
Naming | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Tanenhaus, Michael K. | 2 |
Aslin, Richard N. | 1 |
Gegg-Harrison, Whitney | 1 |
Gorman, Kristen S. | 1 |
Marsh, Chelsea R. | 1 |
McMurray, Bob | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
New York | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Gorman, Kristen S.; Gegg-Harrison, Whitney; Marsh, Chelsea R.; Tanenhaus, Michael K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
When referring to named objects, speakers can choose either a name ("mbira") or a description ("that gourd-like instrument with metal strips"); whether the name provides useful information depends on whether the speaker's knowledge of the name is shared with the addressee. But, how do speakers determine what is shared? In 2…
Descriptors: Experiments, Language Processing, Cognitive Processes, Memory
McMurray, Bob; Tanenhaus, Michael K.; Aslin, Richard N. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Spoken word recognition shows gradient sensitivity to within-category voice onset time (VOT), as predicted by several current models of spoken word recognition, including TRACE (McClelland, J., & Elman, J. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. "Cognitive Psychology," 18, 1-86). It remains unclear, however, whether this sensitivity is…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Inhibition, Auditory Perception, Word Recognition