Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Elementary School Students | 5 |
Grade 4 | 5 |
Elementary School Mathematics | 3 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
Numbers | 3 |
Addition | 2 |
Grade 6 | 2 |
High Achievement | 2 |
Intelligence Tests | 2 |
Mathematics Achievement | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
European Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Educational… | 1 |
Journal of Numerical Cognition | 1 |
Mathematical Thinking and… | 1 |
ZDM: The International… | 1 |
Author
Verschaffel, Lieven | 5 |
Van Dooren, Wim | 3 |
Bakker, Merel | 2 |
De Smedt, Bert | 2 |
Degrande, Tine | 2 |
Torbeyns, Joke | 2 |
Janssen, Rianne | 1 |
Pelgrims, Elise | 1 |
Van Hoof, Jo | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 5 |
Reports - Research | 4 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 4 |
Grade 4 | 3 |
Intermediate Grades | 3 |
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Primary Education | 2 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Audience
Location
Belgium | 3 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 2 |
Block Design Test | 1 |
Need for Cognition Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Bakker, Merel; Pelgrims, Elise; Torbeyns, Joke; Verschaffel, Lieven; De Smedt, Bert – Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2023
While symbolic number processing is an important correlate for typical and low mathematics achievement, it remains to be determined whether children with high mathematics achievement also have excellent symbolic number processing abilities. We investigated this question in 64 children (aged 8 to 10), i.e., 32 children with persistent high…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Numbers, High Achievement, Mathematics Achievement
Bakker, Merel; Torbeyns, Joke; Verschaffel, Lieven; De Smedt, Bert – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2022
Many studies in the past decades have focused on low and typical mathematics achievers, yet little is known about children with high mathematics achievement, particularly at a young age. The current study aimed to fill this gap and started from the early work of Krutetskii (1976) as a theoretical lens to study the characteristics of high…
Descriptors: High Achievement, Elementary School Students, Elementary School Mathematics, Mathematics Achievement
Degrande, Tine; Verschaffel, Lieven; Van Dooren, Wim – European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2018
While previous studies mainly focused on children's additive and multiplicative reasoning abilities, we studied third to sixth graders' "preference" for additive or multiplicative relations. This was investigated by means of schematic problems that were "open" to both types of relations, namely arrow schemes containing three…
Descriptors: Addition, Multiplication, Mathematical Logic, Student Attitudes
Van Hoof, Jo; Janssen, Rianne; Verschaffel, Lieven; Van Dooren, Wim – ZDM: The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 2015
While understanding rational numbers forms an essential part of mathematical literacy, research has repeatedly shown that they form a stumbling block in education. A big source of difficulty is the inappropriate application of natural number knowledge. The research literature points at three main aspects where natural number knowledge is…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Grade 4, Inhibition, Elementary School Mathematics
Degrande, Tine; Verschaffel, Lieven; Van Dooren, Wim – Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal, 2017
In contrast to previous studies on Spontaneous Focusing on Quantitative Relations (SFOR), the present study investigated not only the "extent" to which children focus on (multiplicative) quantitative relations, but also the "nature" of children's quantitative focus (i.e., the types of quantitative relations that children focus…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Grade 2, Grade 4, Grade 6