NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 11 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kemp, Charles; Shafto, Patrick; Tenenbaum, Joshua B. – Cognitive Psychology, 2012
Humans routinely make inductive generalizations about unobserved features of objects. Previous accounts of inductive reasoning often focus on inferences about a single object or feature: accounts of causal reasoning often focus on a single object with one or more unobserved features, and accounts of property induction often focus on a single…
Descriptors: Generalization, Logical Thinking, Inferences, Probability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Endress, Ansgar D.; Wood, Justin N. – Cognitive Psychology, 2011
When other individuals move, we interpret their movements as discrete, hierarchically-organized, goal-directed actions. However, the mechanisms that integrate visible movement features into actions are poorly understood. Here, we consider two sequence learning mechanisms--transitional probability-based (TP) and position-based encoding…
Descriptors: Memory, Probability, Sequential Learning, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Foley, Nicholas C.; Grossberg, Stephen; Mingolla, Ennio – Cognitive Psychology, 2012
How are spatial and object attention coordinated to achieve rapid object learning and recognition during eye movement search? How do prefrontal priming and parietal spatial mechanisms interact to determine the reaction time costs of intra-object attention shifts, inter-object attention shifts, and shifts between visible objects and covertly cued…
Descriptors: Priming, Cues, Reaction Time, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Browning, N. Andrew; Grossberg, Stephen; Mingolla, Ennio – Cognitive Psychology, 2009
Visually-based navigation is a key competence during spatial cognition. Animals avoid obstacles and approach goals in novel cluttered environments using optic flow to compute heading with respect to the environment. Most navigation models try either explain data, or to demonstrate navigational competence in real-world environments without regard…
Descriptors: Optics, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability, Schemata (Cognition)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gao, Tao; Newman, George E.; Scholl, Brian J. – Cognitive Psychology, 2009
Psychologists have long been captivated by the perception of animacy--the fact that even simple moving shapes may appear to engage in animate, intentional, and goal-directed movements. Here we report several new types of studies of a particularly salient form of perceived animacy: "chasing", in which one shape (the "wolf") pursues another shape…
Descriptors: Cues, Inferences, Case Studies, Research Methodology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Magen, Hagit; Cohen, Asher – Cognitive Psychology, 2007
We combine the Dimension-Action (DA) model with translational models to account for both the Stroop and the flanker effects. The basic assumption of the model is that there are distinct visual modules, each of which is endowed with both perception and response selection processes. We contrast this model with an alternative widespread view, the…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Cognitive Processes, Experiments, Color
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Keane, Brian P.; Pylyshyn, Zenon W. – Cognitive Psychology, 2006
In a series of five experiments, we investigated whether visual tracking mechanisms utilize prediction when recovering multiple reappearing objects. When all objects abruptly disappeared and reappeared mid-trajectory, it was found that (a) subjects tracked better when objects reappeared at their loci of disappearance than when they reappeared in…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Cues, Cognitive Processes, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Welder, Andrea N.; Graham, Susan A. – Cognitive Psychology, 2006
In five experiments, 14- to 15-month-old infants' categorization of objects on the basis of more or less obvious features was investigated. Using an object examining paradigm, a total of 200 infants were familiarized with novel objects that shared either more obvious features (i.e., easily visible) or less obvious features (i.e., accessible by…
Descriptors: Infants, Cues, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Baguley, Thom; Lansdale, Mark W.; Lines, Lorna K.; Parkin, Jennifer K. – Cognitive Psychology, 2006
This paper studies the dynamics of attempting to access two spatial memories simultaneously and its implications for the accuracy of recall. Experiment 1 demonstrates in a range of conditions that two cues pointing to different experiences of the same object location produce little or no higher recall than that observed with a single cue.…
Descriptors: Cues, Experiments, Recall (Psychology), Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rastle, Kathleen; Brysbaert, Marc – Cognitive Psychology, 2006
For over 15 years, masked phonological priming effects have been offered as evidence that phonology plays a leading role in visual word recognition. The existence of these effects--along with their theoretical implications--has, however, been disputed. The authors present three sources of evidence relevant to an assessment of the existence and…
Descriptors: Phonology, Word Recognition, English, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Needham, Amy; Cantlon, Jessica F.; Ormsbee Holley, Susan M. – Cognitive Psychology, 2006
The current research investigates infants' perception of a novel object from a category that is familiar to young infants: key rings. We ask whether experiences obtained outside the lab would allow young infants to parse the visible portions of a partly occluded key ring display into one single unit, presumably as a result of having categorized it…
Descriptors: Infants, Investigations, Visual Perception, Classification