Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 58 |
Descriptor
Experiments | 51 |
Animals | 39 |
Conditioning | 23 |
Stimuli | 20 |
Cues | 16 |
Learning Processes | 16 |
Reinforcement | 10 |
Classical Conditioning | 9 |
Context Effect | 9 |
Evaluation Methods | 9 |
Animal Behavior | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Learning and Motivation | 58 |
Author
Papini, Mauricio R. | 5 |
Vervliet, Bram | 4 |
Rosas, Juan M. | 3 |
Abad, Maria J. F. | 2 |
Baeyens, Frank | 2 |
Bouton, Mark E. | 2 |
Cohen, Jerome | 2 |
Grant, Douglas S. | 2 |
Hermans, Dirk | 2 |
Neumann, David L. | 2 |
Pellegrini, Santiago | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 58 |
Reports - Research | 50 |
Reports - Evaluative | 8 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ortega, Leonardo A.; Daniel, Alan M.; Davis, Jessica B.; Fuchs, Perry N.; Papini, Mauricio R. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Physical pain (induced by tissue damage) and psychological pain (induced by surprising incentive loss) share a set of common neural substrates, but little is known about their interactions. The present research studied such interactions using the formalin test to induce physical pain and consummatory successive negative contrast (cSNC) to induce…
Descriptors: Evidence, Pain, Drinking, Psychology
Singer, Rebecca A.; Zentall, Thomas R. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Pigeons prefer a positive discriminative (S+) stimulus that follows a less preferred event (a large number of required responses, a longer delay, or the absence of food) over a different S+ with a similar history of reinforcement that follows a more preferred event (a single required response, no delay, or food). We proposed that this phenomenon…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Experiments, Animals, Stimuli
Grant, Douglas S. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Experiments 1 and 2 involved independent groups that received primary reinforcement after a correct match with a probability of 1.0, 0.50 or 0.25. Correct matches that did not produce primary reinforcement produced a conditioned reinforcer. Both experiments revealed little evidence that acquisition or retention was adversely affected by use of…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Probability, Laboratory Experiments, Conditioning
Keller, Matthew R.; Brown, Michael F. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Pairs of rats foraged in trials either together or separately in an open field apparatus for pellets hidden in discreet locations in a 5 x 5 matrix. Trial duration was either 1 or 4 min. The tendency to choose locations that had earlier been visited by another rat was examined by comparing the choices made in the presence and absence of the other…
Descriptors: Animals, Memory, Spatial Ability, Comparative Analysis
Vervliet, Bram; Iberico, Carlos; Vervoort, Ellen; Baeyens, Frank – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Generalization gradients have been investigated widely in animal conditioning experiments, but much less so in human predictive learning tasks. Here, we apply the experimental design of a recent study on conditioned fear generalization in humans (Lissek et al., 2008) to a predictive learning task, and examine the effects of a number of relevant…
Descriptors: Animals, Research Design, Testing, Conditioning
Rodriguez, Gabriel; Alonso, Gumersinda – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Three conditioned suppression experiments examined the Hall-Pearce (1979) negative transfer effect in rats. Experiment 1 replicated the effect: CS-US[subscript weak] pairings retarded subsequent fear conditioning to the CS as a result of CS-US[subscript strong] pairings. The size of this retardation was less than that produced by non-reinforced CS…
Descriptors: Transfer of Training, Inhibition, Animals, Experiments
Kundey, Shannon M. A.; Strandell, Brittany; Mathis, Heather; Rowan, James D. – Learning and Motivation, 2010
(Hulse and Dorsky, 1977) and (Hulse and Dorsky, 1979) found that rats, like humans, learn sequences following a simple rule-based structure more quickly than those lacking a rule-based structure. Through two experiments, we explored whether two additional species--domesticated horses ("Equus callabus") and chickens ("Gallus domesticus")--would…
Descriptors: Horses, Experiments, Animals, Models
Effting, Marieke; Vervliet, Bram; Kindt, Merel – Learning and Motivation, 2010
Using a conditioned suppression task, two experiments examined retrospective revaluation effects after serial compound training in a release from overshadowing design. In Experiment 1, serial X [right arrow] A+ training produced suppression to target A, which was enhanced when preceded by feature X, whereas X by itself elicited no suppression.…
Descriptors: Experiments, Task Analysis, Evaluation Methods, Cues
Alessandri, Jerome; Stolarz-Fantino, Stephanie; Fantino, Edmund – Learning and Motivation, 2011
A concurrent-chains procedure was used to examine choice between segmented (two-component chained schedules) and unsegmented schedules (simple schedules) in terminal links with equal inter-reinforcement intervals. Previous studies using this kind of experimental procedure showed preference for unsegmented schedules for both pigeons and humans. In…
Descriptors: Science Activities, Intervals, Research Methodology, Reinforcement
Zentall, Thomas R.; Stagner, Jessica P. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Pigeons show a preference for an alternative that provides them with discriminative stimuli (sometimes a stimulus that predicts reinforcement and at other times a stimulus that predicts the absence of reinforcement) over an alternative that provides them with nondiscriminative stimuli, even if the nondiscriminative stimulus alternative is…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Behavior, Reinforcement, Probability
Braaten, Richard F. – Learning and Motivation, 2010
Male zebra finches learn to sing songs that they hear between 25 and 65 days of age, the sensitive period for song learning. In this experiment, male and female zebra finches were exposed to zebra finch songs either before (n = 9) or during (n = 4) the sensitive period. Following song exposure, recognition memory for the songs was assessed with an…
Descriptors: Singing, Recognition (Psychology), Experiments, Memory
Vervliet, Bram; Vansteenwegen, Deb; Hermans, Dirk – Learning and Motivation, 2010
Extinction is generally more fragile than conditioning, as illustrated by the contextual renewal effect. The traditional extinction procedure entails isolated presentations of the conditioned stimulus. Extinction may be boosted by adding isolated presentations of the unconditioned stimulus, as this should augment breaking the contingency between…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Learning Processes, Context Effect, Stimuli
Leon, Samuel P.; Abad, Maria J. F.; Rosas, Juan M. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Four experiments explored the role of contexts in information retrieval after different levels of acquisition training in human predictive learning. Participants were trained where cue (X) was followed by an outcome in context A while a different cue (Y) was followed by the absence of the outcome in context B. When 4 training trials with each cue…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Organizations (Groups), Information Retrieval, Experiments
Diaz, Estrella; De la Casa, L. G. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
This paper presents evidence of extinction, spontaneous recovery and renewal in a conditioned preferences paradigm based on taste-taste associations. More specifically, in three experiments rats exposed to a simultaneous compound of citric acid-saccharin solution showed a preference for the citric solution when the preference was measured with a…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Models, Animals, Laboratory Experiments
Winterbauer, Neil E.; Bouton, Mark E. – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Three experiments with rat subjects examined resurgence of an extinguished instrumental response using the procedure introduced by Epstein (1983) with pigeons. There were three phases: (1) initial acquisition of pressing on a lever (L1) for pellet reward, (2) extinction of L1, and (3) a test session in which a second lever (L2) was inserted,…
Descriptors: Rewards, Experimental Psychology, Animals, Laboratory Experiments