Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Source
Journal of Cognition and… | 2 |
British Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Educational… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Mathematical Thinking and… | 1 |
Mathematics Education… | 1 |
South African Journal of… | 1 |
Author
Geary, David C. | 2 |
Arndt, Dominique | 1 |
Barros, Rossana | 1 |
Bevan, Kate | 1 |
Bryant, Peter | 1 |
Coffman, Jennifer L. | 1 |
Croswell, Melissa | 1 |
Evans, Deborah | 1 |
Fritz, Annemarie | 1 |
Gervasoni, Ann | 1 |
Hadden, Teresa | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 8 |
Journal Articles | 7 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 3 |
Primary Education | 3 |
Grade 1 | 2 |
Grade 2 | 2 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Grade 4 | 2 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
British Ability Scales | 1 |
Wechsler Individual… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Scalise, Nicole R.; Ramani, Geetha B. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2021
Young children's symbolic magnitude understanding, or knowledge of how written numerals and number words can be ordered and compared, is thought to play an important role in their mathematical development. There is consistent evidence that symbolic magnitude skills predict mathematical achievement in later childhood and adulthood. Yet less is…
Descriptors: Predictor Variables, Symbols (Mathematics), Mathematics Skills, Mathematics Achievement
Hudson, Kesha N.; Coffman, Jennifer L.; Ornstein, Peter A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2018
Data from a longitudinal investigation were used to examine the effects of mothers' and teachers' language on children's developing mathematical competencies during the kindergarten year. Specifically, 1) mothers' use of metamemory talk, or references to the process of remembering, and 2) teachers' use of "cognitive processing language"…
Descriptors: Kindergarten, Mothers, Preschool Teachers, Addition
Geary, David C.; Nicholas, Alan; Li, Yaoran; Sun, Jianguo – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2017
The contributions of domain-general abilities and domain-specific knowledge to subsequent mathematics achievement were longitudinally assessed (n = 167) through 8th grade. First grade intelligence and working memory and prior grade reading achievement indexed domain-general effects, and domain-specific effects were indexed by prior grade…
Descriptors: Longitudinal Studies, Mathematics Achievement, Knowledge Level, Developmental Stages
Gervasoni, Ann; Parish, Linda; Livesey, Carole; Croswell, Melissa; Bevan, Kate; Hadden, Teresa; Turkenburg, Kathie – Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, 2013
The longitudinal progress of 42 Grade 1 students who participated in a 10-20 week Extending Mathematical Understanding (EMU) intervention program was examined to evaluate the effectiveness of the program for enhancing and accelerating mathematics learning. Overall the students made accelerated progress during Grade 1 and their learning was…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Elementary School Students, Primary Education, Longitudinal Studies
Nunes, Terezinha; Bryant, Peter; Evans, Deborah; Barros, Rossana – Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal, 2015
Before starting school, many children reason logically about concepts that are basic to their later mathematical learning. We describe a measure of quantitative reasoning that was administered to children at school entry (mean age 5.8 years) and accounted for more variance in a mathematical attainment test than general cognitive ability 16 months…
Descriptors: Young Children, Thinking Skills, Logical Thinking, Concept Formation
Geary, David C.; Hoard, Mary K.; Nugent, Lara – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Children's (N = 275) use of retrieval, decomposition (e.g., 7 = 4+3 and thus 6+7 = 6+4+3), and counting to solve additional problems was longitudinally assessed from first grade to fourth grade, and intelligence, working memory, and in-class attentive behavior was assessed in one or several grades. The goal was to assess the relation between…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Mathematics Achievement, Short Term Memory, Grade 4
Arndt, Dominique; Sahr, Katleen; Opfermann, Maria; Leutner, Detlev; Fritz, Annemarie – South African Journal of Childhood Education, 2013
Recent studies showed that kindergarten children solve addition, subtraction, doubling and halving problems using the core system for the approximate representation of numerical magnitude. In Study 1, 34 first-grade students in their first week of schooling solved approximate arithmetic problems in a number range up to 100 regarding all four basic…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Mathematics Skills, Grade 1, Elementary School Students
Throndsen, Inger – British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2011
Background: Several studies have examined young primary school children's use of strategies when solving simple addition and subtraction problems. Most of these studies have investigated students' strategy use as if they were isolated processes. To date, we have little knowledge about how math strategies in young students are related to other…
Descriptors: Self Efficacy, Academic Achievement, Program Effectiveness, Mathematics Skills