NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pomerantz, James R.; Portillo, Mary C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Gestalt phenomena are often so powerful that mere demonstrations can confirm their existence, but Gestalts have proven hard to define and measure. Here we outline a theory of basic Gestalts (TBG) that defines Gestalts as emergent features (EFs). The logic relies on discovering wholes that are more discriminable than are the parts from which they…
Descriptors: Visual Discrimination, Vision, Visual Stimuli, Undergraduate Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gori, Monica; Tinelli, Francesca; Sandini, Giulio; Cioni, Giovanni; Burr, David – Neuropsychologia, 2012
Multisensory integration of spatial information occurs late in childhood, at around eight years (Gori, Del Viva, Sandini, & Burr, 2008). For younger children, the haptic system dominates size discrimination and vision dominates orientation discrimination: the dominance may reflect "sensory calibration," and could have direct consequences on…
Descriptors: Vision, Visual Discrimination, Spatial Ability, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gligorovic, Milica; Vucinic, Vesna; Eskirovic, Branka; Jablan, Branka – Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2011
This research was conducted in order to examine the influence of manifest strabismus and stereoscopic vision on non-verbal abilities of visually impaired children aged between 7 and 15. The sample included 55 visually impaired children from the 1st to the 6th grade of elementary schools for visually impaired children in Belgrade. RANDOT stereotest…
Descriptors: Vision, Nonverbal Ability, Visual Impairments, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kelso, Robert P., Sr. – Engineering Design Graphics Journal, 2008
The paper presents a unique approach in associating perspective projection with the image beheld by the eye and demonstrates that all graphical and photographic perspective projections must contain distortion when compared to the image beheld by the eye. (Contains 8 figures.)
Descriptors: Human Body, Vision, Visual Perception, Visual Discrimination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hayward, Carol M.; Gromko, Joyce Eastlund – Journal of Research in Music Education, 2009
The purpose of this study was to examine predictors of music sight-reading ability. The authors hypothesized that speed and accuracy of music sight-reading would be predicted by a combination of aural pattern discrimination, spatial-temporal reasoning, and technical proficiency. Participants (N = 70) were wind players in concert bands at a…
Descriptors: Music Reading, Musicians, Vision, Visualization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Fockert, Jan; Davidoff, Jules; Fagot, Joel; Parron, Carole; Goldstein, Julie – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
The Ebbinghaus (Titchener) illusion was examined in a remote culture (Himba) with no words for geometric shapes. The illusion was experienced less strongly by Himba compared with English participants, leading to more accurate size contrast judgments in the Himba. The study included two conditions of inducing stimuli. The illusion was weaker when…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Visual Discrimination, Misconceptions, Cross Cultural Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Minini, Loredana; Jeffery, Kathryn J. – Learning & Memory, 2006
Visual discrimination tasks are increasingly used to explore the neurobiology of vision in rodents, but it remains unclear how the animals solve these tasks: Do they process shapes holistically, or by using low-level features such as luminance and angle acuity? In the present study we found that when discriminating triangles from squares, rats did…
Descriptors: Animals, Cognitive Processes, Visual Stimuli, Visual Perception