Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Speech Communication | 2 |
Video Technology | 2 |
Accuracy | 1 |
Adults | 1 |
Animals | 1 |
Associative Learning | 1 |
Attribution Theory | 1 |
Coding | 1 |
College Students | 1 |
Computational Linguistics | 1 |
Contrastive Linguistics | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Language Learning and… | 2 |
Author
Cameron-Faulkner, Thea R. | 1 |
Gelman, Susan A. | 1 |
Meyer, Meredith | 1 |
Okuno, Akiko | 1 |
Stilwell, Sarah M. | 1 |
Theakston, Anna L. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Okuno, Akiko; Cameron-Faulkner, Thea R.; Theakston, Anna L. – Language Learning and Development, 2020
Languages differ in how they encode causal events, placing greater or lesser emphasis on the agent or patient of the action. Little is known about how these preferences emerge and the relative influence of cognitive biases and language-specific input at different stages in development. In these studies, we investigated the emergence of sentence…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Contrastive Linguistics, Preferences, Linguistic Input
Meyer, Meredith; Gelman, Susan A.; Stilwell, Sarah M. – Language Learning and Development, 2015
Generic noun phrases, or generics, refer to abstract categories ("Dogs" bark) rather than particular individuals ("Those dogs" bark). Study 1 investigated how parents use gestures in association with generic versus particular reference during naturalistic interactions with their 2- and 3-year-old children. Parents provided…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Undergraduate Students, Nouns