Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 9 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 15 |
Descriptor
Color | 20 |
Foreign Countries | 19 |
Visual Stimuli | 7 |
Comparative Analysis | 6 |
Reaction Time | 6 |
Cues | 4 |
Student Attitudes | 4 |
Teaching Methods | 4 |
Undergraduate Students | 4 |
Accuracy | 3 |
College Students | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Kinoshita, Sachiko | 3 |
Mills, Luke | 2 |
Norris, Dennis | 2 |
Arnold, Derek H. | 1 |
Beesley, Tom | 1 |
Boeing, Alexandra | 1 |
Bradshaw, J. L. | 1 |
Brown, Francesca | 1 |
Burnham, D. K. | 1 |
Calder, Andrew J. | 1 |
Cruz, Merlyne | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 20 |
Journal Articles | 18 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 2 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 2 |
Non-Print Media | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 10 |
Postsecondary Education | 8 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Australia | 20 |
Iran | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
New Zealand | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
South Korea | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 3 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
English, Lyn D. – Statistics Education Research Journal, 2023
This article reports on a study in which third-grade students (8-9 years) were given a degree of agency in conducting chance experiments and representing the outcomes. Students chose their own samples of 12 coloured counters, ensuring all colours were represented. They predicted the outcomes of item selection, tested their predictions, explained…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Elementary School Students, Color, Probability
Krajewski, Sabine; Khoury, Matthew – Learning and Teaching: The International Journal of Higher Education in the Social Sciences, 2021
In this article, we argue that physical rooms cannot be replaced by virtual space without literally losing the student's body and that experimenting with rooms and active learning is imperative for improving and advancing students' learning. Our case study offers insight into a 'soft room experiment' without hard furniture or audio-visual…
Descriptors: Educational Environment, Active Learning, Space Utilization, Higher Education
Kinoshita, Sachiko; Mills, Luke – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
The present study investigated how response mode (oral vs. manual) modulates the Stroop effect using a picture variant of the Stroop task in which participants named orally, or identified with a manual keypress, line drawings of animals (e.g., camel). Consistent with previous color-response Stroop studies, relative to the nonlinguistic neutral…
Descriptors: Phonology, Language Processing, Animals, Color
Kinoshita, Sachiko; Mills, Luke; Norris, Dennis – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Using the oral and manual Stroop tasks we tested the claim that retrieval of meaning from a written word is automatic, in the sense that it cannot be controlled. The semantic interference effect (greater interference caused by color-related words than color-neutral words) was used as the index of semantic activation. To manipulate the level of…
Descriptors: Semantics, Color, Interference (Learning), Visual Stimuli
Beesley, Tom; Hanafi, Gunadi; Vadillo, Miguel A.; Shanks, David R.; Livesey, Evan J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Two experiments examined biases in selective attention during contextual cuing of visual search. When participants were instructed to search for a target of a particular color, overt attention (as measured by the location of fixations) was biased strongly toward distractors presented in that same color. However, when participants searched for…
Descriptors: Attention, Cues, Bias, Visual Perception
Kinoshita, Sachiko; De Wit, Bianca; Norris, Dennis – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
In 2 variants of the color-word Stroop task, we compared 5 types of color-neutral distractors--real words (e.g., "HAT"), pseudowords (e.g., "HIX"), consonant strings (e.g., "HDK"), symbol strings (e.g., #$%), and a row of Xs (e.g., "XXX")--as well as incongruent color words (e.g., "GREEN" displayed…
Descriptors: Color, Interference (Learning), Reaction Time, Visual Stimuli
Zazzi, Hannah; Faragher, Rhonda – International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 2018
Objectives: To date, there has been little qualitative research exploring how students interpret visual sensory input in the classroom. Research has found that seeking student voice has the capacity to act as a change agent for Educational Quality of Life (EQOL), in several aspects of educational decision-making. In light of this knowledge, this…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Classroom Environment, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Motamed, Bahareh; Tucker, Richard – International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 2018
This paper sets out to consider the relationship between design education, architects' colour knowledge, colour orientations and colour use in design practice. Specifically, a survey of 274 architects, architectural academics and postgraduates in Australia and Iran addressed the questions--is design education informing colour knowledge, and does…
Descriptors: Color, Design, Correlation, Architecture
Ollerhead, Sue – Language and Education, 2019
Despite the growing numbers of migrant students enrolling in Australian secondary schools, and an official acknowledgment of their complex support and learning needs, there has been little policy focus on the pedagogical changes that need to be made by teachers to accommodate these needs. There is also little understanding of the depth and…
Descriptors: Semiotics, Multilingualism, Student Needs, Cultural Capital
Rhodes, Gillian; Jeffery, Linda; Boeing, Alexandra; Calder, Andrew J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Despite the discovery of body-selective neural areas in occipitotemporal cortex, little is known about how bodies are visually coded. We used perceptual adaptation to determine how body identity is coded. Brief exposure to a body (e.g., anti-Rose) biased perception toward an identity with opposite properties (Rose). Moreover, the size of this…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Human Body, Color, Photography
Irons, Jessica L.; Folk, Charles L.; Remington, Roger W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Although models of visual search have often assumed that attention can only be set for a single feature or property at a time, recent studies have suggested that it may be possible to maintain more than one attentional control setting. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether spatial attention could be guided by multiple attentional…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Spatial Ability, Color, Priming
Ecker, Ullrich K. H.; Maybery, Murray; Zimmer, Hubert D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2013
There is ongoing debate concerning the mechanisms of feature binding in working memory. In particular, there is controversy regarding the extent to which these binding processes are automatic. The present article demonstrates that binding mechanisms differ depending on whether the to-be-integrated features are perceived as forming a coherent…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Color, Geometric Concepts, Cognitive Processes
Srinivasan, Prasanna; Cruz, Merlyne – Pedagogy, Culture and Society, 2015
This article draws on data from qualitative research that was conducted with children between 6 and 13?years of age as a part of an 18-month project that explored respectfully engaging with cultural diversity in Australian primary schools. We used persona dolls as a research tool to enable children to explore and articulate their knowledge of…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Elementary School Students, Ethnic Diversity, Cultural Awareness
Arnold, Derek H.; Wegener, Signy V.; Brown, Francesca; Mattingley, Jason B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Grapheme-color synesthesia is an atypical condition in which individuals experience sensations of color when reading printed graphemes such as letters and digits. For some grapheme-color synesthetes, seeing a printed grapheme triggers a sensation of color, but "hearing" the name of a grapheme does not. This dissociation allowed us to…
Descriptors: Memory, Color, Experimental Psychology, Graphemes
Flaherty, Mary; Moran, Aidan – American Annals of the Deaf, 2007
Most studies on the Stroop effect (unintentional automatic word processing) have been restricted to English speakers using vocal responses. Little is known about this effect with deaf signers. The study compared Stroop task responses among four different samples: deaf participants from a Japanese-language environment and from an English-language…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Language Processing, Deafness, Sign Language
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2