NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Milojevich, H.; Lukowski, A. – Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2016
Background: Whereas research has indicated that children with Down syndrome (DS) imitate demonstrated actions over short delays, it is presently unknown whether children with DS recall information over lengthy delays at levels comparable with typically developing (TD) children matched on developmental age. Method: In the present research, 10…
Descriptors: Down Syndrome, Recall (Psychology), Comparative Analysis, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Parks, Colleen M.; Yonelinas, Andrew P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
It is often assumed that recollection is necessary to support memory for novel associations, whereas familiarity supports memory for single items. However, the levels of unitization framework assumes that familiarity can support associative memory under conditions in which the components of an association are unitized (i.e., treated as a single…
Descriptors: Memory, Familiarity, Cognitive Processes, Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Waismeyer, Anna S.; Jacobs, Lucia F. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
The development of spatial navigation in children depends not only on remembering which landmarks lead to a goal location but also on developing strategies to deal with changes in the environment or imperfections in memory. Using cue combination methods, the authors examined 3- and 4-year-old children's memory for different types of spatial cues…
Descriptors: Cues, Young Children, Memory, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hays, Matthew Jensen; Kornell, Nate; Bjork, Robert A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Teachers and trainers often try to prevent learners from making errors, but recent findings (e.g., Kornell, Hays, & Bjork, 2009) have demonstrated that tests can potentiate subsequent learning even when the correct answer is difficult or impossible to generate (e.g., "What is Nate Kornell's middle name?"). In 3 experiments, we…
Descriptors: Testing, Role, Failure, Semantics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Storm, Benjamin C.; Angello, Genna; Bjork, Elizabeth Ligon – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Research on retrieval-induced forgetting has shown that retrieval can cause the forgetting of related or competing items in memory (Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994). In the present research, we examined whether an analogous phenomenon occurs in the context of creative problem solving. Using the Remote Associates Test (RAT; Mednick, 1962), we…
Descriptors: Animals, Stimuli, Problem Solving, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Parks, Colleen M.; Murray, Linda J.; Elfman, Kane; Yonelinas, Andrew P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Whether recollection is a threshold or signal detection process is highly controversial, and the controversy has centered in part on the shape of receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) and z-transformed ROCs (zROCs). U-shaped zROCs observed in tests thought to rely heavily on recollection, such as source memory tests, have provided evidence in…
Descriptors: Evidence, Video Technology, Sentences, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Rosegard, Erik; Wilson, Jackson – Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 2013
College students ("n" = 846) enrolled in a general education course were randomly assigned to either an arousal (experimental) or no-arousal (control) group. The experimental group was exposed to a topic-relevant, 90-second external stimulus (a technique used to elevate arousal and focus attention). The control group listened to the…
Descriptors: Experimental Groups, Control Groups, College Students, Lecture Method