Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Arithmetic | 6 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Experiments | 3 |
Memory | 3 |
Short Term Memory | 3 |
College Students | 2 |
Elementary School Students | 2 |
Models | 2 |
Multiplication | 2 |
Psychology | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 6 |
Belgium | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Xu, Chang; LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina; Maloney, Erin A.; Wylie, Judith; Simms, Victoria; Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn; Osana, Helena P. – Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2023
Children's knowledge of the ordinal relations among number symbols is related to their mathematical learning. Ordinal knowledge has been measured using judgment (i.e., decide whether a sequence of three digits is in order) and ordering tasks (i.e., order three digits from smallest to largest). However, the question remains whether performance on…
Descriptors: Young Children, Numeracy, Number Concepts, Serial Ordering
Campbell, Jamie I. D.; Thompson, Valerie A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF) is a widely studied phenomenon of human memory, but RIF of arithmetic facts remains relatively unexplored. In 2 experiments, we investigated RIF of simple addition facts (2 + 3 = 5) from practice of their multiplication counterparts (2 x 3 = 6). In both experiments, robust RIF expressed in response times occurred…
Descriptors: Evidence, Semantics, Memory, Multiplication
Campbell, Jamie I. D.; Alberts, Nicole M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
Educated adults solve simple addition problems primarily by direct memory retrieval, as opposed to by counting or other procedural strategies, but they report using retrieval substantially less often with problems in written-word format (four = eight) compared with digit format (4 = 8). It was hypothesized that retrieval efficiency is relatively…
Descriptors: Subtraction, Information Retrieval, Costs, Memory
Imbo, Ineke; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
In the present study, the authors tested the effects of working-memory load on math problem solving in 3 different cultures: Flemish-speaking Belgians, English-speaking Canadians, and Chinese-speaking Chinese currently living in Canada. Participants solved complex addition problems (e.g., 58 + 76) in no-load and working-memory load conditions, in…
Descriptors: Cultural Differences, Arithmetic, Problem Solving, Short Term Memory
Campbell, Jamie I. D.; Robert, Nicole D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
A variety of experimental evidence indicates that the memory representation for multiplication facts (e.g., 6 [times] 9 = 54) incorporates bidirectional links with a forward association from factors to product and a reverse association from product to factors. Surprisingly, the authors did not find evidence in Experiment 1 of facilitative…
Descriptors: Memory, Multiplication, Experiments, Arithmetic

Brainerd, Charles J. – Child Development, 1983
Presents a stochastic model for distinguishing mental arithmetic errors according to causes of failure. A series of experiments (1) studied questions of goodness of fit and model validity among four and five year olds and (2) used the model to measure the relative contributions of developmental improvements in short-term memory and arithmetical…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Early Childhood Education