Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Foreign Countries | 5 |
Problem Solving | 5 |
Reaction Time | 5 |
Adults | 2 |
Beliefs | 2 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Inferences | 2 |
Probability | 2 |
Undergraduate Students | 2 |
Validity | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Thompson, Valerie A. | 2 |
Bernfeld, Gary A. | 1 |
Christoph, Gabriela | 1 |
Gibb, Maia | 1 |
Goldhammer, Frank | 1 |
Handley, Simon J. | 1 |
Imbo, Ineke | 1 |
LeFevre, Jo-Anne | 1 |
Lüdtke, Oliver | 1 |
Martens, Thomas | 1 |
Newman, Ian R. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 5 |
Journal Articles | 3 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Postsecondary Education | 3 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 5 |
Australia | 1 |
Austria | 1 |
Belgium | 1 |
Cyprus | 1 |
Czech Republic | 1 |
Denmark | 1 |
Estonia | 1 |
Finland | 1 |
France | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Matching Familiar Figures Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Newman, Ian R.; Gibb, Maia; Thompson, Valerie A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
It is commonly assumed that belief-based reasoning is fast and automatic, whereas rule-based reasoning is slower and more effortful. Dual-Process theories of reasoning rely on this speed-asymmetry explanation to account for a number of reasoning phenomena, such as base-rate neglect and belief-bias. The goal of the current study was to test this…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Beliefs, Bias, Problem Solving
Goldhammer, Frank; Martens, Thomas; Christoph, Gabriela; Lüdtke, Oliver – OECD Publishing, 2016
In this study, we investigated how empirical indicators of test-taking engagement can be defined, empirically validated, and used to describe group differences in the context of the Programme of International Assessment of Adult Competences (PIAAC). The approach was to distinguish between disengaged and engaged response behavior by means of…
Descriptors: International Assessment, Adults, Response Style (Tests), Reaction Time
Pennycook, Gordon; Trippas, Dries; Handley, Simon J.; Thompson, Valerie A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Base-rate neglect refers to the tendency for people to underweight base-rate probabilities in favor of diagnostic information. It is commonly held that base-rate neglect occurs because effortful (Type 2) reasoning is required to process base-rate information, whereas diagnostic information is accessible to fast, intuitive (Type 1) processing…
Descriptors: Probability, Intuition, Cognitive Processes, Physicians
Xu, Chang; Wells, Emma; LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Imbo, Ineke – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The purpose of the present study was to examine factors that influence strategic flexibility in computational estimation for Chinese- and Canadian-educated adults. Strategic flexibility was operationalized as the percentage of trials on which participants chose the problem-based procedure that best balanced proximity to the correct answer with…
Descriptors: Computation, Problem Solving, Feedback (Response), Accuracy
Peters, R. DeV.; Bernfeld, Gary A. – 1980
The major purpose of this study was to assess the degree to which cognitive style, as indexed by the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT), is related to the way in which children respond to problems with social or interpersonal content. Fifty-two reflective and 64 impulsive first- and second-grade boys were presented with a number of stories in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style, Comparative Analysis, Conceptual Tempo