Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Reaction Time | 6 |
Attention | 4 |
Visual Perception | 3 |
Visual Stimuli | 3 |
College Students | 2 |
Congruence (Psychology) | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
Perception | 2 |
Perception Tests | 2 |
Undergraduate Students | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Henik, Avishai | 3 |
Arend, Isabel | 1 |
Benoni, Hanna | 1 |
Dimov, Eduard | 1 |
Ganel, Tzvi | 1 |
Goldfarb, Liat | 1 |
Kalanthroff, Eyal | 1 |
Meiran, Nachshon | 1 |
Taha, Haitham | 1 |
Tsal, Yehoshua | 1 |
Weinbach, Noam | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Israel | 6 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Taha, Haitham – Reading Psychology, 2023
The capacities of detecting visual regularities were tested among twenty typical (age 11.1 ± 0.32), and twenty poor (age 11.03 ± 0.28) native-Arab readers. Two stages were implemented, passive exposure to visual regularities and forced decision task. In the first stage, the participants were passively presented with four shapes; each shape was…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Early Adolescents, Visual Stimuli, Reaction Time
Arend, Isabel; Henik, Avishai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
The size congruity effect (SiCE) shows that number and physical size interact as magnitudes. That is, response times are faster when number and size are congruent (e.g., 2 4) than when they are incongruent (e.g., 2 4). A shared representational system has been the most influential account for the SiCE. Recently, this account has been challenged by…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Reaction Time, Geometric Concepts, Interaction
Meiran, Nachshon; Dimov, Eduard; Ganel, Tzvi – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
In the present experiments, the question being addressed was whether switching attention between perceptual dimensions and selective attention to dimensions are processes that compete over a common resource? Attention to perceptual dimensions is usually studied by requiring participants to ignore a never-relevant dimension. Selection failure…
Descriptors: Attention, Perception, Cognitive Ability, Cues
Kalanthroff, Eyal; Goldfarb, Liat; Henik, Avishai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Performance of the Stroop task reflects two conflicts--informational (between the incongruent word and ink color) and task (between relevant color naming and irrelevant word reading). The task conflict is usually not visible, and is only seen when task control is damaged. Using the stop-signal paradigm, a few studies demonstrated longer…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Color, Naming, Word Recognition
Tsal, Yehoshua; Benoni, Hanna – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
The substantial distractor interference obtained for small displays when the target appears alone is reduced in large displays when the target is embedded among neutral letters. This finding has been interpreted as reflecting low-load and high-load processing, respectively, thereby supporting the theory of perceptual load (Lavie & Tsal, 1994).…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Attention, Perception, Memory
Weinbach, Noam; Henik, Avishai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The current study focuses on the relationship between alerting and executive attention. Previous studies reported an increased flanker congruency effect following alerting cues. In the first two experiments, we found that the alertness-congruency interaction did not exist for all executive tasks (it appeared for a flanker task but not for a Stroop…
Descriptors: Attention, Executive Function, Spatial Ability, Cues