Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 13 |
Descriptor
Dimensional Preference | 44 |
Visual Perception | 44 |
Infants | 14 |
Visual Stimuli | 13 |
Visual Discrimination | 12 |
Color | 10 |
Spatial Ability | 10 |
Perceptual Development | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 8 |
Attention | 7 |
Age Differences | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Abbott, Sara | 1 |
Berger, Carole | 1 |
Bhatt, Ramesh S. | 1 |
Bornstein, Marc H. | 1 |
Bower, T. G. R. | 1 |
Brannon, Elizabeth M. | 1 |
Cantlon, Jessica F. | 1 |
Cho, Yang Seok | 1 |
Cole, Geoff | 1 |
Colombo, John | 1 |
Danoff, Jerome V. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 44 |
Reports - Research | 31 |
Reports - Evaluative | 6 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - General | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 9 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Location
France | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Michel, Carine; Quercia, Patrick; Joubert, Lise – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 2019
To better identify the distinctive characteristics of space representation in the radial dimension, we have proposed a new paradigm: the landmarks alignment task where two parallel aluminum bars were radially presented. Children had to move a landmark along one bar and place it at the same location as the reference landmark placed by the examiner…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Spatial Ability, Children, Dyslexia
Loucks, Jeff; Sommerville, Jessica A. – Child Development, 2012
Recent evidence suggests adults and infants selectively attend to features of action, such as how a hand contacts an object. The current research investigated whether this bias stems from infants' processing of the functional consequences of grasps: understanding that different grasps afford different future actions. A habituation paradigm…
Descriptors: Role, Psychomotor Skills, Infants, Visual Perception
Woods, Adam J.; Philbeck, John W.; Danoff, Jerome V. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
D. R. Proffitt and colleagues (e. g., D. R. Proffitt, J. Stefanucci, T. Banton, & W. Epstein, 2003) have suggested that objects appear farther away if more effort is required to act upon them (e.g., by having to throw a ball). The authors attempted to replicate several findings supporting this view but found no effort-related effects in a variety…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception, Dimensional Preference

May, Jo Whitten; May, J. Gaylord – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1979
Findings showed no preference for the color white over the color black, contrary to previous studies. (Author/DLS)
Descriptors: Color, Dimensional Preference, Infants, Visual Perception

West, Robin L.; Odom, Richard D. – Child Development, 1979
Kindergarten children were given a salience-assessment task to determine each child's salience hierarchy for the dimensions of form, color, and position, and each was provided perceptual training with his/her least salient dimension. Training promoted fewer errors in recall in comparison to control group subjects. (RH)
Descriptors: Dimensional Preference, Kindergarten Children, Recall (Psychology), Training
Tzur, Boaz; Frost, Ram – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Applying Bloch's law to visual word recognition research, both exposure duration of the prime and its luminance determine the prime's overall energy, and consequently determine the size of the priming effect. Nevertheless, experimenters using fast-priming paradigms traditionally focus only on the SOA between prime and target to reflect the…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Cognitive Processes, Word Recognition, Research Problems
Gellatly, Angus; Pilling, Michael; Cole, Geoff; Skarratt, Paul – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
Object substitution masking (OSM) is said to occur when a perceptual object is hypothesized that is mismatched by subsequent sensory evidence, leading to a new hypothesized object being substituted for the first. For example, when a brief target is accompanied by a longer lasting display of nonoverlapping mask elements, reporting of target…
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Perceptual Development, Dimensional Preference, Visual Perception

Wagner, Sheldon; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Eight sets of paired auditory and visual stimuli were constructed. Each member of the auditory pair was matched by one member of the visual pair (e.g., ascending "tone/up arrow"; descending "tone/down arrow"). Sixty-one infants with a mean age of 11.4 months were presented matching and unmatching stimuli; total fixation time…
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Auditory Stimuli, Dimensional Preference, Infants

Withrow, Rebecca L. – Journal of Humanistic Counseling, Education and Development, 2004
This article reviews the published literature on the separate fields of art therapy and color therapy, synthesizing them in a proposed use of color within art therapy. Specific techniques focusing on use of color in a nonrepresentational expressive form are suggested as a way to extend the therapeutic benefits of art therapy. The intention of this…
Descriptors: Art Therapy, Color, Counseling Techniques, Emotional Response
Strother, Lars; Kubovy, Michael – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
The authors conducted 3 experiments to explore the roles of curvature, density, and relative proximity in the perceptual organization of ambiguous dot patterns. To this end, they developed a new family of regular dot patterns that tend to be perceptually grouped into parallel contours, dot-sampled structured grids (DSGs). DSGs are similar to the…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Proximity, Visual Perception, Experimental Psychology

Yonas, Albert; And Others – Child Development, 1987
A test for sensitivity to binocular disparity and a shape perception test were administered to four-month-olds. Results indicated that disparity-sensitive infants could perceive three-dimensional-object shape from kinetic and binocular depth information. (PCB)
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Dimensional Preference, Eye Fixations, Infants

Maurer, Daphne; and Adams, Russell J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1987
Two different methods which minimize achromatic cues were used to test the ability of one-month-olds to discriminate gray from broadband blue. Test data imply an improvement between birth and one month of age in the discrimination of gray from broadband blue. Possible physiological changes underlying this improvement are discussed. (Author/RWB)
Descriptors: Color, Dimensional Preference, Infants, Visual Discrimination

Bower, T. G. R.; And Others – Science, 1979
A previously-reported experiment designed to determine if newborn infants can distinguish between an object and a picture of that object is flawed. The experimental design and an improved design are discussed. (BB)
Descriptors: Dimensional Preference, Perception, Perceptual Development, Research

Rivera, Susan M.; Wakeley, Ann; Langer, Jonas – Developmental Psychology, 1999
Two experiments investigated whether 5-month olds would look longer at rotating "drawbridge" appearing to violate physical laws because they knew it was causally impossible. Findings indicated that infants' longer gaze at 180-degree rotations was due to simple perceptual preference for more motion, challenging Baillargeon's (1987) claim…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Dimensional Preference, Habituation, Infant Behavior

Bhatt, Ramesh S.; Waters, Susan E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1998
Three experiments examined infants' processing of three-dimensional (3D) information in static images. Results indicated that 3-month olds are sensitive to 3D cues in static images. However, discrepancies based on these cues may not engage infants' attention like those based on fundamental features. (Author)
Descriptors: Attention, Cues, Dimensional Preference, Infant Behavior