Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Generalization | 10 |
Stimulus Generalization | 10 |
Stimuli | 3 |
Adults | 2 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Behavior | 2 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Learning | 2 |
Logical Thinking | 2 |
Models | 2 |
Patterned Responses | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Arantes, Joana | 1 |
Barnes-Holmes, Dermot | 1 |
Carr, James E. | 1 |
Doyle, Patrick J. | 1 |
Dry, Matthew J. | 1 |
Gunby, Kristin V. | 1 |
Ivy, Jonathan W. | 1 |
Lasenby, Jennifer | 1 |
LeBlanc, Linda A. | 1 |
Lee, Michael D. | 1 |
Lee, Naomi | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Australia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meindl, James N.; Ivy, Jonathan W.; Miller, Neal; Neef, Nancy A.; Williamson, Robert L. – Journal of Behavioral Education, 2013
Fluency-based strategies such as Say All Fast a Minute Each Day Shuffled (SAFMEDS) effectively promote fluent responding (i.e., high rate and accuracy). It is possible, however, that the stimulus control developed through these activities inhibits stimulus generalization. We investigated this concern in a two-part study with college students.…
Descriptors: Generalization, Instructional Materials, College Students, Verbal Stimuli
Navarro, Daniel J.; Dry, Matthew J.; Lee, Michael D. – Cognitive Science, 2012
Inductive generalization, where people go beyond the data provided, is a basic cognitive capability, and it underpins theoretical accounts of learning, categorization, and decision making. To complete the inductive leap needed for generalization, people must make a key "sampling" assumption about how the available data were generated.…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Generalization, Sampling, Learning
Arantes, Joana; Machado, Armando – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
In the present study we extended errorless learning to a conditional temporal discrimination. Pigeons' responses to a left-red key after a 2-s sample and to a right-green key after a 10-s sample were reinforced. There were two groups: One learned the discrimination through trial and error and the other through an errorless learning procedure.…
Descriptors: Stimulus Generalization, Generalization, Comparative Analysis, Animals
Gunby, Kristin V.; Carr, James E.; LeBlanc, Linda A. – Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2010
Three children with autism were taught abduction-prevention skills using behavioral skills training with in situ feedback. All children acquired the skills, which were maintained at a 1-month follow-up assessment. In addition, 1 of the children demonstrated the skills during a stimulus generalization probe in a community setting. (Contains 1…
Descriptors: Autism, Prevention, Stimulus Generalization, Child Safety
Matos, Maria Amelia; Passos, Maria de Lourdes – Behavior Analyst, 2010
The production of verbal operants not previously taught is an important aspect of language productivity. For Skinner, new mands, tacts, and autoclitics result from the recombination of verbal operants. The relation between these mands, tacts, and autoclitics is what linguists call "analogy," a grammatical pattern that serves as a foundation on…
Descriptors: Creativity, Verbal Stimuli, Grammar, Linguistics

Doyle, Patrick J.; And Others – Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1987
The effect of syntax training on the sentence production of four adults with Broca's aphasia was examined. Generalization and maintenance were measured, and naive judges rated "adequacy" of responses. Findings indicated that effects are limited to the grammatical constructions taught, and that effects on response adequacy may be limited.…
Descriptors: Adults, Aphasia, Generalization, Instructional Effectiveness

Shepard, Roger N. – Science, 1987
Describes the establishment of a psychological space for any set of stimuli by determining metric distances between the stimuli with the probability that a response learned for a stimulus will generalize to the other. (Author/TW)
Descriptors: College Science, Conditioning, Generalization, Higher Education

Morrongiello, Barbara A.; Lasenby, Jennifer; Lee, Naomi – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2003
Two studies examined the impact of temporal synchrony on infants' learning of and memory for sight-sound pairs. Findings indicated that 7-month-olds had no difficulty learning auditory-visual pairs regardless of temporal synchrony, remembering them 10 minutes later and 1 week later. Three-month-olds showed poorer learning in no-synchrony than in…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comparative Analysis, Cross Sectional Studies, Generalization

Smeets, Paul M.; Barnes-Holmes, Dermot; Roche, Bryan – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Trained preschoolers and adults on three sets of successive discriminations with stimuli labeled A, B, and R. Tested for derived stimulus-response relations and stimulus-stimulus relations. Adults displayed class-consistent B-R and A-B performances over all conditions. Children's display of class-consistent B-R performance varied by training…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Concept Formation, Discrimination Learning

Pepitone, Albert; Triandis, Harry C. – Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1987
There is no a priori basis for assuming that theories of social psychology are universal. For theories to be universal, the meaning of the stimulus would have to be consistent across cultures, but this cannot be taken for granted. Cross-cultural variations in social behavior may be "surface" expressions of deep structure norms that are universal.…
Descriptors: Behavior Standards, Behaviorism, Cross Cultural Studies, Cultural Context