Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Ideography | 9 |
Language Processing | 8 |
Chinese | 6 |
Language Research | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Japanese | 3 |
Word Recognition | 3 |
Classification | 2 |
College Students | 2 |
English | 2 |
Experiments | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
Journal of Learning… | 1 |
Journal of Psycholinguistic… | 1 |
Journal of the Chinese… | 1 |
Language Learning | 1 |
Language and Speech | 1 |
Reading and Writing: An… | 1 |
Visible Language | 1 |
Author
Chambers, Craig G. | 1 |
Chan, Won Shing Raymond | 1 |
Chu-Chang, Mae | 1 |
Crutcher, Keith A. | 1 |
Everson, Michael E. | 1 |
Feldman, Laurie B. | 1 |
Gordon, Peter C. | 1 |
Hino, Yasushi | 1 |
Hung, Se Fong | 1 |
Jared, Debra | 1 |
Kusunose, Yuu | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hino, Yasushi; Kusunose, Yuu; Lupker, Stephen J.; Jared, Debra – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Studies using the lexical decision task with English stimuli have demonstrated that homophones are responded to more slowly than nonhomophonic controls. In contrast, several studies using Chinese stimuli have shown that homophones are responded to more rapidly than nonhomophonic controls. In an attempt to better understand the impact of homophony,…
Descriptors: Phonology, Languages, Differences, Language Processing
Tsang, Cara; Chambers, Craig G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Cantonese shape classifiers encode perceptual information that is characteristic of their associated nouns, although certain nouns are exceptional. For example, the classifier "tiu" occurs primarily with nouns for long-narrow-flexible objects (e.g., scarves, snakes, and ropes) and also occurs with the noun for a (short, rigid) key. In 3…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Comprehension, Semantics, Nouns
Lee, Yoonhyoung; Nam, Kichun; Gordon, Peter C. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2009
Korean writing is a syllabary where spaces occur between phrases rather than between words. This characteristic of Korean allows different types of information in Korean sentences to be dissociated in ways that are not possible in the languages that have been the focus of most psycholinguistic research, thereby providing new opportunities to…
Descriptors: Sentences, Language Processing, Korean, Morphology (Languages)
Cognitive Profiling in Chinese Developmental Dyslexia with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders
Chan, Won Shing Raymond; Hung, Se Fong; Liu, Suet Nga; Lee, Cheuk Kiu Kathy – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2008
The cognitive profiles of children with Developmental Reading Disorder (RD) and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders (ADHD) have been extensively studied in alphabetic language communities. Deficits in phonological processing and rapid naming have been implicated as core features of RD although whether the latter is a deficit specific to RD…
Descriptors: Reading Difficulties, Dyslexia, Attention Deficit Disorders, Hyperactivity

Feldman, Laurie B.; Turvey, M. T. – Language and Speech, 1980
When two Japanese adults named colors written in Kanji, a logographic orthography, and in Kana, a syllabary, the latency to vocalization was consistently less for Kana. This superiority of Kana is attributed to the closer relation of Kana to phonology and, therefore, to speech. (Author/RL)
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Graphemes, Ideography, Japanese

Everson, Michael E. – Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association, 1988
First-year students of Chinese at the United States Air Force Academy (n=60) were tested for reading speed and comprehension of Chinese passages presented either in characters or romanization. Students read faster and understood more in romanization than in characters. (LMO)
Descriptors: Chinese, College Students, Higher Education, Ideography

Sheridan, E. Marcia – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1983
The question of whether it is easier to learn to read through an ideographic, syllabic, or alphabetic writing system is posed. The linguistic nature of Chinese, Japanese, Korean, and English are examined in relation to differences in information processing and cultural factors related to reading disability. (Author/SEW)
Descriptors: Beginning Reading, Chinese, Cultural Differences, English
Zender, Mike; Crutcher, Keith A. – Visible Language, 2007
The accelerating rate of data generation and resulting publications are taxing the ability of scientific investigators to stay current with the emerging literature. This problem, acute in science, is not uncommon in other areas. New approaches to managing this explosion of information are needed. While it is only possible to read one paper or…
Descriptors: Alzheimers Disease, Pathology, Scientific Concepts, Research Methodology

Chu-Chang, Mae; Loritz, Donald J. – Language Learning, 1977
Twenty-two Cantonese-speaking Chinese students and 16 Spanish-speaking students were tested for short-term memory encoding strategies on word-recognition tests. Chinese speakers were found to encode Chinese ideographs phonologically, but both Chinese and Spanish learners of English were found to encode English words visually. (Author/CFM)
Descriptors: Bilingual Students, Chinese, English (Second Language), Ideography