Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Motion | 14 |
Visual Perception | 7 |
Spatial Ability | 6 |
Visual Stimuli | 6 |
Experimental Psychology | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Perceptual Motor Coordination | 3 |
Bias | 2 |
Color | 2 |
Computation | 2 |
Evaluation Methods | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 14 |
Author
Ambadar, Zara | 1 |
Boker, Steven M. | 1 |
Brenner, Eli | 1 |
Brick, Timothy R. | 1 |
Bundesen, Claus | 1 |
Cohn, Jeffrey F. | 1 |
Collier, Geoffrey L. | 1 |
Dale, Rick | 1 |
Dienes, Zoltan | 1 |
Fajen, Brett R. | 1 |
Flombaum, Jonathan I. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 14 |
Reports - Descriptive | 14 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Florida | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hock, Howard S.; Nichols, David F. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
A version of the line motion illusion (LMI) occurs when one of two adjacent surfaces changes in luminance; a new surface is perceived sliding in front of the initially presented surface. Previous research has implicated high-level mechanisms that can create or modulate LMI motion via feedback to lower-level motion detectors. It is shown here that…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Perception, Visual Stimuli
Do Curved Reaching Movements Emerge from Competing Perceptions? A Reply to van der Wel et al. (2009)
Spivey, Michael J.; Dale, Rick; Knoblich, Guenther; Grosjean, Marc – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Spivey, Grosjean, and Knoblich (2005) reported smoothly curved reaching movements, via computer-mouse tracking, which suggested a continuously evolving flow of distributed lexical activation patterns into motor movement during a phonological competitor task. For example, when instructed to click the "candy," participants' mouse-cursor trajectories…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Language Research, Language Processing, Phonology
Boker, Steven M.; Cohn, Jeffrey F.; Theobald, Barry-John; Matthews, Iain; Mangini, Michael; Spies, Jeffrey R.; Ambadar, Zara; Brick, Timothy R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
During conversation, women tend to nod their heads more frequently and more vigorously than men. An individual speaking with a woman tends to nod his or her head more than when speaking with a man. Is this due to social expectation or due to coupled motion dynamics between the speakers? We present a novel methodology that allows us to randomly…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Speech Communication, Motion, Sexual Identity
Kyllingsbaek, Soren; Bundesen, Claus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
Observers given brief exposures of pairs of colored bars and asked to report both the color and the orientation of each bar showed evidence of stochastic independence between reports of the 4 features (2 colors and 2 orientations). The authors also found virtually perfect stochastic independence between reports of colors and directions of motion…
Descriptors: Motion, Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli, Probability
Brenner, Eli; van Beers, Robert J.; Rotman, Gerben; Smeets, Jeroen B. J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
It only makes sense to talk about the position of a moving object if one specifies the time at which its position is of interest. The authors here show that when a flash or tone specifies the moment of interest, subjects estimate the moving object to be closer to where it passes the fixation point and further in its direction of motion than it…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Motion, Bias, Visual Perception
Collier, Geoffrey L.; Ogden, R. Todd – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2004
The Wing-Kristofferson model (A. M. Wing & A. B. Kristofferson, (1973a, 1973b) decomposes the variance of isochronous finger tapping into 2 components: a central clock component and a peripheral motor component. The method assumes that there is no drift in the intertap intervals. A new method is introduced that further decomposes the clock…
Descriptors: Intervals, Psychological Studies, Evaluation Methods, Time
Rock, Paul B.; Harris, Mike G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
D. N. Lee (1976) described a braking strategy based on optical expansion in which the driver brakes so that the target's time-to-contact declines around a constant slope in the range -0.5 less than or equal to tau less than 0. The present results from a series of braking simulations confirm and extend earlier reports (E. H. Yilmaz & W. H. Warren,…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Performance, Reaction Time, Perceptual Motor Coordination
Flombaum, Jonathan I.; Scholl, Brian J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
Meaningful visual experience requires computations that identify objects as the same persisting individuals over time, motion, occlusion, and featural change. This article explores these computations in the tunnel effect: When an object moves behind an occluder, and then an object later emerges following a consistent trajectory, observers…
Descriptors: Computation, Color, Motion, Memory
McLeod, Peter; Reed, Nick; Dienes, Zoltan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
The generalized optic acceleration cancellation (GOAC) theory of catching proposes that the path of a fielder running to catch a ball is determined by the attempt to satisfy 2 independent constraints. The 1st is to keep the angle of elevation of gaze to the ball increasing at a decreasing rate. The 2nd is to control the rate of horizontal rotation…
Descriptors: Optics, Physics, Motion, Simulation
Ivanoff, Jason; Klein, Raymond M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to a mechanism that results in a performance disadvantage typically observed when targets are presented at a location once occupied by a cue. Although the time course of the phenomenon--from the cue to the target--has been well studied, the time course of the effect--from target to response--is unknown. In 2…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Reaction Time, Cues, Cognitive Processes
Krampe, Ralf Th.; Mayr, Ulrich; Kliegl, Reinhold – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
The authors demonstrate that the timing and sequencing of target durations require low-level timing and executive control. Sixteen young (M-sub(age) = 19 years) and 16 older (M-sub(age) = 70 years) adults participated in 2 experiments. In Experiment 1, individual mean-variance functions for low-level timing (isochronous tapping) and the sequencing…
Descriptors: Young Adults, Older Adults, Motion, Psychomotor Skills
Wilkie, Richard M.; Wann, John P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
During locomotion, retinal flow, gaze angle, and vestibular information can contribute to one's perception of self-motion. Their respective roles were investigated during active steering: Retinal flow and gaze angle were biased by altering the visual information during computer-simulated locomotion, and vestibular information was controlled…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Spatial Ability, Psychomotor Skills, Error Patterns
Marken, Richard S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
D. M. Shaffer and M. K. McBeath (see record 2002-02027-006) plotted the optical trajectories of uncatchable fly balls and concluded that linear optical trajectory is the informational basis of the actions taken to catch these balls. P. McLeod, N. Reed, and Z. Dienes (see record 2002-11140-016) replotted these trajectories in terms of changes in…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Optics, Geometric Concepts, Motion
Fajen, Brett R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
Braking to avoid a collision can be controlled by keeping the deceleration required to stop (i.e., ideal deceleration) in the "safe" region below maximum deceleration, but maximum deceleration is not optically specified and can vary as conditions change. When brake strength was manipulated between participants using a simulated braking task, the…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Traffic Safety, Visual Perception, Cognitive Processes