Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Behavioral Science Research | 4 |
Time | 4 |
Animal Behavior | 3 |
Animals | 3 |
Probability | 2 |
Reinforcement | 2 |
Academic Achievement | 1 |
Associative Learning | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
Cooperation | 1 |
Correlation | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of the Experimental… | 6 |
Author
Baum, William M. | 1 |
Critchfield, Thomas S. | 1 |
Hale, Sandra | 1 |
Kessel, Robert | 1 |
Lejeune, Helga | 1 |
Lucke, Robert L. | 1 |
Mace, F. Charles | 1 |
Myerson, Joel | 1 |
Rachlin, Howard | 1 |
Richelle, Marc | 1 |
Wearden, J. H. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Descriptive | 6 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Baum, William M. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Choice may be defined as the allocation of behavior among activities. Since all activities take up time, choice is conveniently thought of as the allocation of time among activities, even if activities like pecking are most easily measured by counting. Since dynamics refers to change through time, the dynamics of choice refers to change of…
Descriptors: Behavioral Science Research, Animals, Animal Behavior, Time
Mace, F. Charles; Critchfield, Thomas S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
"Pure basic" science can become detached from the natural world that it is supposed to explain. "Pure applied" work can become detached from fundamental processes that shape the world it is supposed to improve. Neither demands the intellectual support of a broad scholarly community or the material support of society. Translational research can do…
Descriptors: Behavioral Science Research, Innovation, Time Perspective, Futures (of Society)
Kessel, Robert; Lucke, Robert L. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
Shull, Gaynor and Grimes advanced a model for interresponse time distribution using probabilistic cycling between a higher-rate and a lower-rate response process. Both response processes are assumed to be random in time with a constant rate. The cycling between the two processes is assumed to have a constant transition probability that is…
Descriptors: Statistical Analysis, Probability, Monte Carlo Methods, Simulation
Williams, Ben; Myerson, Joel; Hale, Sandra – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
Despite its avowed goal of understanding individual behavior, the field of behavior analysis has largely ignored the determinants of consistent differences in level of performance among individuals. The present article discusses major findings in the study of individual differences in intelligence from the conceptual framework of a functional…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Individual Differences, Short Term Memory, Behavioral Science Research
Lejeune, Helga; Richelle, Marc; Wearden, J. H. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
The article discusses two important influences of B. F. Skinner, and later workers in the behavior-analytic tradition, on the study of animal timing. The first influence is methodological, and is traced from the invention of schedules imposing temporal constraints or periodicities on animals in "The Behavior of Organisms," through the rate…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research, Scheduling
Rachlin, Howard – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
In general, if a variable can be expressed as a function of its own maximum value, that function may be called a discount function. Delay discounting and probability discounting are commonly studied in psychology, but memory, matching, and economic utility also may be viewed as discounting processes. When they are so viewed, the discount function…
Descriptors: Memory, Probability, Theories, Economics