Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Tests | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
Recall (Psychology) | 2 |
Task Analysis | 2 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Associative Learning | 1 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
Decision Making | 1 |
Geometry | 1 |
Inferences | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Atkinson, Janette | 1 |
Braddick, Oliver J. | 1 |
Diana, Rachel A. | 1 |
Knowland, Victoria C. P. | 1 |
Nardini, Marko | 1 |
Reder, Lynne M. | 1 |
Thomas, Rhiannon L. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Nardini, Marko; Thomas, Rhiannon L.; Knowland, Victoria C. P.; Braddick, Oliver J.; Atkinson, Janette – Cognition, 2009
Reorientation tasks, in which disoriented participants attempt to relocate objects using different visual cues, have previously been understood to depend on representing aspects of the global organisation of the space, for example its major axis for judgements based on geometry. Careful analysis of the visual information available for these tasks…
Descriptors: Cues, Spatial Ability, Task Analysis, Inferences
Diana, Rachel A.; Reder, Lynne M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
Low-frequency words produce more hits and fewer false alarms than high-frequency words in a recognition task. The low-frequency hit rate advantage has sometimes been attributed to processes that operate during the recognition test (e.g., L. M. Reder et al., 2000). When tasks other than recognition, such as recall, cued recall, or associative…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Word Recognition, Cognitive Tests, Recall (Psychology)