Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Classification | 10 |
Children | 4 |
Adults | 3 |
Child Development | 3 |
Cognitive Development | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Experimental Groups | 2 |
Infants | 2 |
Preschool Children | 2 |
Visual Stimuli | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 10 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Evaluative | 10 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Eisenberg, Nancy; VanSchyndel, Sarah K.; Spinrad, Tracy L. – Child Development, 2016
Because motivations for prosocial actions typically are unclear, sometimes even to actors but especially for observers, it is difficult to study prosocial motivation. This article reviews research that provides evidence regarding children's motives for prosocial behaviors. First, we present a heuristic model to classify motives on the dimension of…
Descriptors: Motivation, Prosocial Behavior, Literature Reviews, Heuristics
Johnson, Kerri L.; Lurye, Leah E.; Tassinary, Louis G. – Child Development, 2010
Two studies examined how children between ages 4 and 6 use body shape (i.e., the waist-to-hip-ratio [WHR]) for sex categorization. In Study 1 (N = 73), 5- and 6-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds, selected bodies with increasingly discrepant WHRs to be "most like a man" and "most like a woman." Similarly, sex category judgments made by 5- and…
Descriptors: Cues, Eye Movements, Preschool Children, Classification
Thompson, Clarissa A.; Opfer, John E. – Child Development, 2010
How does understanding the decimal system change with age and experience? Second, third, sixth graders, and adults (Experiment 1: N = 96, mean ages = 7.9, 9.23, 12.06, and 19.96 years, respectively) made number line estimates across 3 scales (0-1,000, 0-10,000, and 0-100,000). Generation of linear estimates increased with age but decreased with…
Descriptors: Experimental Groups, Grade 6, Grade 2, Age Differences
Casasola, Marianella; Bhagwat, Jui – Child Development, 2007
Eighteen-month-olds' spatial categorization was tested when hearing a novel spatial word. Infants formed an abstract categorical representation of support (i.e., placing 1 object on another) when hearing a novel spatial particle during habituation but not when viewing the events in silence. Infants with a productive spatial vocabulary did not…
Descriptors: Nouns, Verbs, Form Classes (Languages), Infants

Hartley, Jeffrey L.; And Others – Child Development, 1982
Three experiments used adults' performances in a category abstraction paradigm to test for the existence of individual style in the drawings of three five-year-olds. Overall, studies provided experimental support for the notion that young children possess recognizable artistic styles. (MP)
Descriptors: Adults, Classification, Creative Art, Freehand Drawing
Horst, Jessica S.; Oakes, Lisa M.; Madole, Kelly L. – Child Development, 2005
Despite a large body of research demonstrating the kinds of categories to which infants respond, few studies have directly assessed how infants' categorization unfolds over time. Four experiments used a visual familiarization task to evaluate 10-month-old infants' (N=98) learning of exemplars characterized by commonalities in appearance or…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes
Fisher, Anna V.; Sloutsky, Vladimir M. – Child Development, 2005
The ability to perform induction appears early; however, underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Some argue that early induction is category based, whereas others suggest that early induction is similarity based. Category- and similarity-based induction should result in different memory traces and thus in different memory accuracy. Performing…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Memory, Children, Age Differences
Gastgeb, Holly Zajac; Strauss, Mark S.; Minshew, Nancy J. – Child Development, 2006
This study examined the effect of exemplar typicality on reaction time and accuracy of categorization. High-functioning children (age 9-12), adolescents (age 13-16), and adults with autism (age 17-48) and matched controls were tested in a category verification procedure. All groups showed improved processing throughout the lifespan for typical and…
Descriptors: Autism, Reaction Time, Classification, Matched Groups
Patterson, Meagan M.; Bigler, Rebecca S. – Child Development, 2006
This study was designed to examine the effects of adults' labeling and use of social groups on preschool children's intergroup attitudes. Children (N=87, aged 3-5) attending day care were given measures of classification skill and self-esteem and assigned to membership in a novel ("red" or "blue") social group. In experimental classrooms, teachers…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Self Esteem, Childhood Attitudes, Classification

Asher, Steven R.; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Critically evaluates in terms of predictive and concurrent validity a widely used method of identifying children at risk in peer relations. This method emphasizes children's total rate of peer interaction, regardless of the quality or skillfulness of the interaction. Recommendations are given for future research in this area. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Children, Classification, Clinical Diagnosis, Human Relations