Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 14 |
Descriptor
Animals | 15 |
Animal Behavior | 9 |
Reinforcement | 7 |
Stimuli | 6 |
Food | 4 |
Behavioral Science Research | 3 |
Conditioning | 3 |
Cues | 3 |
Experiments | 3 |
Models | 3 |
Responses | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of the Experimental… | 15 |
Author
Arantes, Joana | 1 |
Bacha-Mendez, Gustavo | 1 |
Benjumea, Santiago | 1 |
Breier, Bernhard H. | 1 |
Chen, Rong | 1 |
Dallery, Jesse | 1 |
Davison, Michael | 1 |
Everly, Jessica B. | 1 |
Fowler, Stephen C. | 1 |
Fraser, Mhoyra | 1 |
Garcia, Andres | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 15 |
Reports - Evaluative | 15 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Everly, Jessica B.; Perone, Michael – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Although response-dependent shock often suppresses responding, response facilitation can occur. In two experiments, we examined the suppressive and facilitative effects of shock by manipulating shock intensity and the interresponse times that produced shock. Rats' lever presses were reinforced on a variable-interval 40-s schedule of food…
Descriptors: Animals, Experiments, Animal Behavior, Punishment
Lazareva, Olga F. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
In a typical transposition task, an animal is presented with a single pair of stimuli (for example, S3+S4-, where plus and minus denote reward and nonreward and digits denote stimulus location on a sensory dimension such as size). Subsequently, an animal is presented with a testing pair that contains a previously reinforced or nonreinforced…
Descriptors: Animals, Stimuli, Reinforcement, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
McDevitt, Margaret A.; Williams, Ben A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Pigeons were presented with a concurrent-chains schedule in which the total time to primary reinforcement was equated for the two alternatives (VI 30 s VI 60 s vs. VI 60 s VI 30 s). In one set of conditions, the terminal links were signaled by the same stimulus, and in another set of conditions they were signaled by different stimuli. Choice was…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reinforcement, Conditioning, Animals
Zarcone, Troy J.; Chen, Rong; Fowler, Stephen C. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
The effect of force requirements on response effort was examined using inbred C57BL/6J mice trained to press a disk with their snout. Lateral peak forces greater than 2 g were defined as responses (i.e., all responses above the measurement threshold). Different, higher force requirements were used to define criterion responses (a subclass of all…
Descriptors: Animals, Reinforcement, Animal Behavior, Evaluation Methods
MacDonall, James S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
This experiment compared descriptions of concurrent choice by the stay/switch model, which says choice is a function of the reinforcers obtained for staying at and for switching from each alternative, and the generalized matching law, which says choice is a function of the total reinforcers obtained at each alternative. For the stay/switch model…
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Comparative Analysis, Models, Animals
Shahan, Timothy A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Stimuli associated with primary reinforcers appear themselves to acquire the capacity to strengthen behavior. This paper reviews research on the strengthening effects of conditioned reinforcers within the context of contemporary quantitative choice theories and behavioral momentum theory. Based partially on the finding that variations in…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reinforcement, Responses, Conditioning
Iversen, Iver H. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
An inexpensive and automated method for presentation of olfactory or tactile stimuli in a two-choice task for rats was implemented with the use of a computer-controlled bidirectional motor. The motor rotated a disk that presented two stimuli of different texture for tactile discrimination, or different odor for olfactory discrimination. Because…
Descriptors: Olfactory Perception, Research Methodology, Training, Cues
Urcuioli, Peter J.; Vasconcelos, Maarco – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
Two experiments examined whether acquired sample equivalence in many-to-one matching was affected by variation in sample-response requirements. In each experiment, pigeons responded on either identical or different response schedules to the sample stimuli that occasioned the same reinforced comparison choice (i.e., to the within-class samples).…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Animals, Responses, Reinforcement
Udell, Monique A. R.; Wynne, C. D. L. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
Dogs likely were the first animals to be domesticated and as such have shared a common environment with humans for over ten thousand years. Only recently, however, has this species' behavior been subject to scientific scrutiny. Most of this work has been inspired by research in human cognitive psychology and suggests that in many ways dogs are…
Descriptors: Cognitive Psychology, Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research
Bacha-Mendez, Gustavo; Reid, Alliston K.; Mendoza-Soylovna, Adela – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
Two experiments with rats examined the dynamics of well-learned response sequences when reinforcement contingencies were changed. Both experiments contained four phases, each of which reinforced a 2-response sequence of lever presses until responding was stable. The contingencies then were shifted to a new reinforced sequence until responding was…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Animals, Responses, Contingency Management
Davison, Michael; Krageloh, Christian U.; Fraser, Mhoyra; Breier, Bernhard H. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
Two groups of 10 male rats were trained to nose poke for food pellets at four alternatives that provided differing rates of pellet delivery on aperiodic schedules. After a fixed number of pellets had been delivered, 5, 10 or 20 in different conditions of the experiment, a 10-s blackout occurred, and the locations of the differing rates of pellet…
Descriptors: Pregnancy, Computation, Nutrition, Mothers
Arantes, Joana; Machado, Armando – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
Pigeons were trained on two temporal bisection tasks, which alternated every two sessions. In the first task, they learned to choose a red key after a 1-s signal and a green key after a 4-s signal; in the second task, they learned to choose a blue key after a 4-s signal and a yellow key after a 16-s signal. Then the pigeons were exposed to a…
Descriptors: Context Effect, Animals, Time, Perception
Swaddle, John P.; Johnson, Charles W. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
Small deviations from bilateral symmetry (fluctuating asymmetries) are cues to fitness differences in some animals. Therefore, researchers have considered whether animals use these small asymmetries as visual cues to determine appropriate behavioral responses (e.g., mate preferences). However, there have been few systematic studies of animals'…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Cues, Visual Discrimination
Garcia, Andres; Benjumea, Santiago – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
In Experiment 1, 10 pigeons were exposed to a successive symbolic matching-to-sample procedure in which the sample was generated by the pigeons' own behavior. Each trial began with both response keys illuminated white, one being the "correct" key and the other the "incorrect" key. The pigeons had no way of discriminating which key was correct and…
Descriptors: Probability, Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research
Soto, Paul L.; McDowell, Jack J.; Dallery, Jesse – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2005
Herrnstein's hyperbola describes the relation between response rate and reinforcer rate on variable-interval (VI) schedules. According to Herrnstein's (1970) interpretation, the parameter "r[subscript e]"represents the reinforcer rate extraneous to the alternative to which the equation is fitted (the target alternative). The hyperbola is based on…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Intervals, Animals, Experiments