Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Error Patterns | 9 |
Experiments | 5 |
Science Instruction | 3 |
Statistical Analysis | 3 |
Teaching Methods | 3 |
Data Analysis | 2 |
Language Processing | 2 |
Science Experiments | 2 |
Accuracy | 1 |
Adults | 1 |
Chemistry | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Ambridge, Ben | 1 |
Berntsen, Oriana | 1 |
Bird, Kevin D. | 1 |
Brenneman, William A. | 1 |
Budden, Katherine | 1 |
Cisek, Richard | 1 |
Folk, Jocelyn R. | 1 |
Girault, Isabelle | 1 |
Heinicke, Susanne | 1 |
Jones, Angela C. | 1 |
Joseph, Ariana | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Evaluative | 9 |
Journal Articles | 8 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Ohio | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Joseph, Ariana; Budden, Katherine; Cisek, Richard; Tokarz, Danielle – Journal of Chemical Education, 2018
In a university third-year instrumental chemistry laboratory students build a laser based polarimeter for determining light scattering with commercially available optical components used in modern optics research laboratories. During this laboratory experiment, students learn that solutions containing molecules which scatter light also influence…
Descriptors: Chemistry, Science Instruction, Undergraduate Students, Error Patterns
Girault, Isabelle; d'Ham, Cédric – Journal of Science Education and Technology, 2014
When solving a scientific problem through experimentation, students may have the responsibility to design the experiment. When students work in a conventional condition, with paper and pencil, the designed procedures stay at a very general level. There is a need for additional scaffolds to help the students perform this complex task. We propose a…
Descriptors: Science Instruction, Problem Solving, Science Experiments, Scaffolding (Teaching Technique)
Heinicke, Susanne – Interchange: A Quarterly Review of Education, 2014
Every measurement in science, every experimental decision, result and information drawn from it has to cope with something that has long been named by the term "error". In fact, errors describe our limitations when it comes to experimental science and science looks back on a long tradition to cope with them. The widely known way to cope…
Descriptors: Coping, Teaching Methods, Motivation Techniques, Science Education History
Bird, Kevin D. – Psychological Methods, 2011
Any set of confidence interval inferences on J - 1 linearly independent contrasts on J means, such as the two comparisons [mu][subscript 1] - [mu][subscript 2] and [mu][subscript 2] - [mu][subscript 3] on 3 means, provides a basis for the deduction of interval inferences on all other contrasts, such as the redundant comparison [mu][subscript 1] -…
Descriptors: Intervals, Statistical Analysis, Inferences, Comparative Analysis
Robinson, Timothy J.; Brenneman, William A.; Myers, William R. – Journal of Statistics Education, 2009
While split-plot designs have received considerable attention in the literature over the past decade, there seems to be a general lack of intuitive understanding of the error structure of these designs and the resulting statistical analysis. Typically, students learn the proper error terms for testing factors of a split-plot design via "expected…
Descriptors: Research Design, Statistical Analysis, Graphs, Intuition
Jones, Angela C.; Folk, Jocelyn R.; Rapp, Brenda – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
A central issue in the study of reading and spelling has been to understand how the consistency or frequency of letter-sound relationships affects written language processing. We present, for the first time, evidence that the sound-spelling frequency of "subgraphemic" elements of words (letters within digraphs) contributes to the…
Descriptors: Spelling, Written Language, Short Term Memory, Language Processing
Ambridge, Ben; Rowland, Caroline F.; Pine, Julian M. – Cognitive Science, 2008
According to Crain and Nakayama (1987), when forming complex yes/no questions, children do not make errors such as "Is the boy who smoking is crazy?" because they have innate knowledge of "structure dependence" and so will not move the auxiliary from the relative clause. However, simple recurrent networks are also able to avoid…
Descriptors: Children, Language Processing, Language Patterns, Linguistic Input
Salmon, Karen; Yao, Joanna; Berntsen, Oriana; Pipe, Margaret-Ellen – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
We investigated the conditions under which preparatory information presented 1 day before a novel event influenced 6-year-olds' recall 1 week later. Children were assigned to one of six experimental conditions. Three conditions involved preparatory information that described the event accurately but differed according to the presence and type of…
Descriptors: Photography, Novels, Recall (Psychology), Experiments
Love, Gloria C. – 1988
The probability of experiment-wise error is explored. Overall, the experiment-wise error rate is directly related to the test-wise error rate--the alpha level set by researchers to curtail the existence of a Type I error. A Type I error occurs when a true null hypothesis is rejected in a given study or experiment. The experiment-wise error rate is…
Descriptors: Data Analysis, Error Patterns, Estimation (Mathematics), Experiments