Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Models | 12 |
Orthographic Symbols | 12 |
Phonology | 6 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Reading Difficulties | 4 |
Reading Processes | 4 |
Semantics | 4 |
Word Recognition | 4 |
Chinese | 3 |
Coding | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Lupker, Stephen J. | 2 |
Baayen, R. Harald | 1 |
Bertand, Daisy | 1 |
Bi, Yanchao | 1 |
Bowers, Jeffrey S. | 1 |
Caramazza, Alfonso | 1 |
Chan, David W. | 1 |
Charles Perfetti | 1 |
Chung, Kevin K. H. | 1 |
Conlon, Elizabeth G. | 1 |
Creed, Peter A. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Evaluative | 12 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
Canada | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lin Chen; Charles Perfetti – Language Teaching Research Quarterly, 2024
Learning new words is fundamental in both first and second-language reading. There are, however, divided opinions on the best instructional approaches. Two widely used approaches across languages are whole-word focus and word-constituent focus. The appropriateness of each approach has varied historically, even within a single language (e.g., the…
Descriptors: Chinese, Second Language Learning, Second Language Instruction, Teaching Methods
Tracey, Diane H. – Education and Urban Society, 2017
Knowing how to provide effective literacy instruction is important for all educators, but it is critically important for urban educators. This article is built on the assumption that deepening urban educators' understanding of the reading process will better equip them to facilitate students' reading development, and to diagnose and intervene if…
Descriptors: Reading Processes, Reading Instruction, Urban Teaching, Faculty Development
Testolin, Alberto; Stoianov, Ivilin; Sperduti, Alessandro; Zorzi, Marco – Cognitive Science, 2016
Learning the structure of event sequences is a ubiquitous problem in cognition and particularly in language. One possible solution is to learn a probabilistic generative model of sequences that allows making predictions about upcoming events. Though appealing from a neurobiological standpoint, this approach is typically not pursued in…
Descriptors: Orthographic Symbols, Neurological Organization, Models, Probability
Yang, Jianfeng; Shu, Hua; McCandliss, Bruce D.; Zevin, Jason D. – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2013
Learning to read in any language requires learning to map among print, sound and meaning. Writing systems differ in a number of factors that influence both the ease and the rate with which reading skill can be acquired, as well as the eventual division of labor between phonological and semantic processes. Further, developmental reading disability…
Descriptors: Reading Skills, Semantics, Reading Difficulties, Chinese
Grainger, Jonathan; Lete, Bernard; Bertand, Daisy; Dufau, Stephane; Ziegler, Johannes C. – Cognition, 2012
We describe a multiple-route model of reading development in which coarse-grained orthographic processing plays a key role in optimizing access to semantics via whole-word orthographic representations. This forms part of the direct orthographic route that gradually replaces phonological recoding during the initial phases of reading acquisition.…
Descriptors: Evidence, Reading Difficulties, Reading, Semantics
Bowers, Jeffrey S.; Damian, Markus F.; Davis, Colin J. – Psychological Review, 2009
A central claim shared by most recent models of short-term memory (STM) is that item knowledge is coded independently from order in long-term memory (LTM; e.g., the letter A is coded by the same representational unit whether it occurs at the start or end of a sequence). Serial order is computed by dynamically binding these item codes to a separate…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Models, Coding, Orthographic Symbols
Nakayama, Mariko; Sears, Christopher R.; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Recent studies have found that masked word primes that are orthographic neighbors of the target inhibit lexical decision latencies (Davis & Lupker, 2006; Nakayama, Sears, & Lupker, 2008), consistent with the predictions of lexical competition models of visual word identification (e.g., Grainger & Jacobs, 1996). In contrast, using the…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Testing, Word Recognition, Orthographic Symbols
Bi, Yanchao; Xu, Yaoda; Caramazza, Alfonso – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2009
One important finding with the picture-word interference paradigm is that picture-naming performance is facilitated by the presentation of a distractor (e.g., CAP) formally related to the picture name (e.g., "cat"). In two picture-naming experiments we investigated the nature of such form facilitation effect with Mandarin Chinese, separating the…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Phonology, Models, Mandarin Chinese
Ho, Connie S.-H.; Chan, David W.; Chung, Kevin K. H.; Lee, Suk-Han; Tsang, Suk-Man – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
The dual-route model offers a popular way to classify developmental dyslexia into phonological and surface subtypes. The current study examined whether this dual-route model could provide a framework for understanding the varieties of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Three groups of Chinese children (dyslexics, chronological-age controls, and…
Descriptors: Models, Evaluation Methods, Dyslexia, Developmental Delays
Perea, Manuel; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Nonwords created by transposing two "adjacent" letters (i.e., transposed-letter (TL) nonwords like "jugde") are very effective at activating the lexical representation of their base words. This fact poses problems for most computational models of word recognition (e.g., the interactive-activation model and its extensions), which assume that exact…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Word Recognition, Models, Lexicology
Conlon, Elizabeth G.; Zimmer-Gembeck, Melanie J.; Creed, Peter A.; Tucker, Melinda – Journal of Research in Reading, 2006
This study evaluated a model of reading skills among early adolescents (N=174). Measures of family history, achievement, cognitive processes and self-perceptions of abilities were obtained. Significant relationships were found between family history and children's single-word reading skills, spelling, reading comprehension, orthographic processing…
Descriptors: Genealogy, Cognitive Ability, Early Adolescents, Reading Skills
Verhoeven, Ludo; Schreuder, Rob; Baayen, R. Harald – Learning and Instruction, 2006
Besides phonotactic principles, orthographies entail graphotactic rules for which the reader must convert a phonological representation on the basis of spelling adaptation rules. In the present study, the learnability of such rules will be investigated with reference to Dutch. Although Dutch orthography can be considered highly regular, there are…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Spelling, Written Language, Indo European Languages