Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 21 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 26 |
Nonverbal Communication | 26 |
Infants | 13 |
Age Differences | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Developmental Stages | 7 |
Autism | 6 |
Toddlers | 6 |
Adults | 5 |
Children | 5 |
Cognitive Development | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Behne, Tanya | 2 |
Carpenter, Malinda | 2 |
Tomasello, Michael | 2 |
Abels, Monika | 1 |
Amatuni, Andrei | 1 |
Barbosa, Fernando | 1 |
Bateman, Amanda | 1 |
Bergelson, Elika | 1 |
Blau, Rivka | 1 |
Borke, Jorn | 1 |
Brannon, Elizabeth M. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Evaluative | 26 |
Journal Articles | 25 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 5 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
California | 1 |
China | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
United States | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Kaufman Assessment Battery… | 1 |
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Moore, Charlotte; Dailey, Shannon; Garrison, Hallie; Amatuni, Andrei; Bergelson, Elika – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Around their first birthdays, infants begin to point, walk, and talk. These abilities are appreciable both by researchers with strictly standardized criteria and caregivers with more relaxed notions of what each of these skills entails. Here, we compare the onsets of these skills and links among them across two data collection methods: observation…
Descriptors: Child Development, Infants, Child Behavior, Vocabulary Development
Understanding the Development of Face and Emotion Processing under a Predictive Processing Framework
Pereira, Mariana R.; Barbosa, Fernando; de Haan, Michelle; Ferreira-Santos, Fernando – Developmental Psychology, 2019
In the present work, we explore the development of processing of emotional facial configurations under a predictive processing (or predictive coding) framework. Predictive processing provides a new approach to brain function that has been used to explain a wide range of processes, from perception to socioemotional processing. The explanatory power…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Nonverbal Communication, Emotional Response, Nonverbal Ability
Gunnarsdottir, Bryndis; Bateman, Amanda – Early Childhood Folio, 2017
Conversation analysis (CA) can be a useful tool for research when investigating toddlers' social interactions, because it can showcase their capabilities and agency through in-depth analysis of their verbal and non-verbal actions. This article argues that by using CA to analyse the details of toddler conversations within the peer group, we can not…
Descriptors: Discourse Analysis, Toddlers, Child Language, Peer Relationship
Gómez, Juan-Carlos – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2015
Children with autism use hand taking and hand leading gestures to interact with others. This is traditionally considered to be an example of atypical behaviour illustrating the lack of intersubjective understanding in autism. However the assumption that these gestures are atypical is based upon scarce empirical evidence. In this paper I present…
Descriptors: Children, Autism, Nonverbal Communication, Observation
Wieder, Serena – Topics in Language Disorders, 2017
Symbolic play is a powerful vehicle for supporting emotional development and communication. It embraces all developmental capacities. This article describes how symbols are formed and how emotional themes are symbolized whereby children reveal their understanding of the world, their feelings and relationships, and how they see themselves in the…
Descriptors: Play, Emotional Response, Models, Child Development
Frazier, Brandy N.; Gelman, Susan A.; Kaciroti, Niko; Russell, Joshua W.; Lumeng, Julie C. – Developmental Science, 2012
This research investigates children's use of social categories in their food selection. Across three studies, we presented preschoolers with sets of photographs that contrasted food-eating models with different characteristics, including model gender, race (Black, White), age (child or adult), and/or expression (acceptance or rejection of the…
Descriptors: Food, Eating Habits, Decision Making, Preschool Children
Vaidya, Chandan J.; Foss-Feig, Jennifer; Shook, Devon; Kaplan, Lauren; Kenworthy, Lauren; Gaillard, William D. – Developmental Science, 2011
Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to examine functional anatomy of attention to social (eye gaze) and nonsocial (arrow) communicative stimuli in late childhood and in a disorder defined by atypical processing of social stimuli, Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Children responded to a target word ("LEFT"/"RIGHT") in the context of a…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Autism, Brain
Pimperton, Hannah; Pellicano, Elizabeth; Jeffery, Linda; Rhodes, Gillian – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
DevDevelopmental improvements in face identity recognition ability are widely documented, but the source of children's immaturity in face recognition remains unclear. Differences in the way in which children and adults visually represent faces might underlie immaturities in face recognition. Recent evidence of a face identity aftereffect (FIAE),…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Human Body, Cognitive Processes, Adults
Fleck, Bethany; Chavajay, Pablo – Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2009
This naturalistic observational study described the similarities and differences in physical interactions involving preschoolers and kindergartners within the context of a US childcare facility. It examined patterns of touch involving the children across center and circle activities within the course of their day. Results indicated that…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Kindergarten, Young Children, Interaction
Gao, Xiaoqing; Maurer, Daphne – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Using 20 levels of intensity, we measured children's thresholds to discriminate the six basic emotional expressions from neutral and their misidentification rates. Combined with the results of a previous study using the same method ("Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102" (2009) 503-521), the results indicate that by 5 years of age,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Development, Emotional Response, Nonverbal Communication
Liebal, Kristin; Behne, Tanya; Carpenter, Malinda; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Science, 2009
We investigated whether 1-year-old infants use their shared experience with an adult to determine the meaning of a pointing gesture. In the first study, after two adults had each shared a different activity with the infant, one of the adults pointed to a target object. Eighteen- but not 14-month-olds responded appropriately to the pointing gesture…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Infants, Language Acquisition, Adults
Blau, Rivka; Klein, Pnina S. – Early Child Development and Care, 2010
In this study, the effects of eliciting positive and negative emotions on various cognitive functions of four- to five-year-old preschool children were examined. Emotions were elicited through presentations of "happy" and "sad" video clips, before the children performed the cognitive tasks. Behavioural (facial expressions) and…
Descriptors: Metabolism, Emotional Response, Preschool Children, Cognitive Processes
Cordes, Sara; Brannon, Elizabeth M. – Developmental Science, 2008
We review recently published papers that have contributed to our understanding of how the preverbal infant represents number, area and time. We review evidence that infants rely on two distinct systems to represent number nonverbally and highlight the similarities in the ratio-dependent discrimination of number, time and area. Contrary to earlier…
Descriptors: Infants, Competence, Concept Formation, Time
Holliday, Erin L.; Harrison, Linda J.; McLeod, Sharynne – Journal of Early Childhood Research, 2009
Including children as research participants is an important new direction in early childhood research. However, it is rare for such studies to include the voices of children with significant communication impairment. This article suggests that drawing may be an appropriate non-verbal method for "listening" to these children's ideas and recording…
Descriptors: Childrens Art, Communication Disorders, Interpersonal Communication, Freehand Drawing
Rowe, Meredith L.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Developmental Science, 2009
The gestures children produce predict the early stages of spoken language development. Here we ask whether gesture is a global predictor of language learning, or whether particular gestures predict particular language outcomes. We observed 52 children interacting with their caregivers at home, and found that gesture use at 18 months selectively…
Descriptors: Sentences, Speech Communication, Caregivers, Language Acquisition
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2