Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Motion | 9 |
Prediction | 9 |
Visual Perception | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Correlation | 2 |
Interaction | 2 |
Physics | 2 |
Visual Stimuli | 2 |
Adolescents | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
British Journal of… | 1 |
Child Development | 1 |
Cognition | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Physics Teacher | 1 |
Psychological Bulletin | 1 |
Psychological Review | 1 |
Research in Developmental… | 1 |
Author
White, Peter A. | 2 |
Amorim, Michel-Ange | 1 |
Aslin, Richard N. | 1 |
Baures, Robin | 1 |
Benguigui, Nicolas | 1 |
Boets, Bart | 1 |
Broderick, Michael P. | 1 |
Cairney, John | 1 |
Chua, Romeo | 1 |
Cicchino, Jessica B. | 1 |
Cornelissen, Piers | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Evaluative | 9 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom (Wales) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Frodyma, Marc – Physics Teacher, 2020
Students have difficulty bridging the conceptual gap between Newtonian and relativistic physics, and, consequently, the teaching of special relativity has been discussed extensively in the literature. A comprehensive list of such references is too large to include, but a brief list is given. In this paper, the author presents several exercises,…
Descriptors: Physics, Scientific Concepts, Science Process Skills, Prediction
White, Peter A. – Psychological Bulletin, 2012
Forces are experienced in actions on objects. The mechanoreceptor system is stimulated by proximal forces in interactions with objects, and experiences of force occur in a context of information yielded by other sensory modalities, principally vision. These experiences are registered and stored as episodic traces in the brain. These stored…
Descriptors: Play, Imagery, Vision, Motion
Cicchino, Jessica B.; Aslin, Richard N.; Rakison, David H. – Cognition, 2011
The associative learning account of how infants identify human motion rests on the assumption that this knowledge is derived from statistical regularities seen in the world. Yet, no catalog exists of what visual input infants receive of human motion, and of causal and self-propelled motion in particular. In this manuscript, we demonstrate that the…
Descriptors: Photography, Cues, Outcomes of Treatment, Infants
Ebersbach, Mirjam; Van Dooren, Wim; Verschaffel, Lieven – International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 2011
The present study aimed at investigating children's and adolescents' understanding of constant and accelerated motions. The main objectives were (1) to investigate whether different task formats would affect the performance and (2) to track developmental changes in this domain. Five to 16 year olds (N = 157) predicted the distances of a moving…
Descriptors: Word Problems (Mathematics), Science Instruction, Task Analysis, Motion
Cairney, John; Hay, John; Veldhuizen, Scott; Faught, Brent – Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2010
Oxygen consumption at peak physical exertion (VO[subscript 2] maximum) is the most widely used indicator of cardiorespiratory fitness. The purpose of this study was to compare two protocols for its estimation, cycle ergometer testing and the 20 m shuttle run, among children with and without probable developmental coordination disorder (pDCD). The…
Descriptors: Physical Fitness, Developmental Disabilities, Measurement Equipment, Human Body
Boets, Bart; Vandermosten, Maaike; Cornelissen, Piers; Wouters, Jan; Ghesquiere, Pol – Child Development, 2011
Evidence suggests that sensitivity to coherent motion (CM) is related to reading, but its role in the etiology of developmental dyslexia remains unclear. In this longitudinal study, CM sensitivity was measured in 31 children at family risk for dyslexia and 31 low-risk controls. Children, diagnosed with dyslexia in third grade (mean age = 8 years 3…
Descriptors: Longitudinal Studies, Dyslexia, Motion, Etiology
White, Peter A. – Psychological Review, 2009
Impressions of force are commonplace in the visual perception of objects interacting. It is proposed that these impressions have their source in haptically mediated experiences of exertion of force in actions on objects. Visual impressions of force in interactions between objects occur by a kind of generalization of the proprioceptive impression…
Descriptors: Causal Models, Visual Perception, Cognitive Psychology, Visual Stimuli
Benguigui, Nicolas; Broderick, Michael P.; Baures, Robin; Amorim, Michel-Ange – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2008
In coincidence-timing studies, children have been shown to respond too early to slower stimuli and too late to faster stimuli. To examine this velocity effect, children aged 6, 7.5, 9, 10.5, and adults were tested with two different velocities in a prediction-motion task which consisted of judging, after the occlusion of the final part of its…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Prediction, Motion, Responses
Morein-Zamir, Sharon; Chua, Romeo; Franks, Ian; Nagelkerke, Paul; Kingstone, Alan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
Using a continuous tracking task, the authors examined whether stopping is resistant to expectancies as well as whether it is a representative measure of response control. Participants controlled the speed of a moving marker by continuously adjusting their response force. Participants stopped their ongoing tracking in response to auditory signals…
Descriptors: Sensory Experience, Psychomotor Skills, Motor Development, Attention Control