Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 12 |
Descriptor
Semantics | 20 |
Written Language | 20 |
Models | 7 |
English | 5 |
Oral Language | 5 |
Syntax | 5 |
Word Recognition | 5 |
Chinese | 4 |
Cognitive Mapping | 4 |
English (Second Language) | 4 |
Language Processing | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Reports - Evaluative | 20 |
Journal Articles | 16 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 4 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 8 | 1 |
Grade 9 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Patience Stevens; David C. Plaut – Grantee Submission, 2022
The morphological structure of complex words impacts how they are processed during visual word recognition. This impact varies over the course of reading acquisition and for different languages and writing systems. Many theories of morphological processing rely on a decomposition mechanism, in which words are decomposed into explicit…
Descriptors: Written Language, Morphology (Languages), Word Recognition, Reading Processes
Al-Saudi, Jibrel Harb; Al-Rawajfeh, Aiman Eid – Educational Research and Reviews, 2022
This study aims to identify the cementing devices used in the Holy Qur'an, Al-Qasas Surah, in particular. Specific cementing devices appear to connect the phrases and clauses contextualized in the verses of this Surah. Three exegeses and three different translations of the meaning of the Holy Qur'an were referred to in this study for data…
Descriptors: Islam, Semitic Languages, Religious Factors, Ambiguity (Semantics)
Patience Stevens; David Plaut – Grantee Submission, 2020
The statistical structure of a given language likely drives our sensitivity to words' morphological structure. The current work begins to investigate to what degree morphological processing effects observed in visual word recognition can be attributed to statistical regularities between orthography and semantics in English, without any prior…
Descriptors: Reading Processes, Word Recognition, Semantics, Written Language
Yang, Jianfeng; Shu, Hua; McCandliss, Bruce D.; Zevin, Jason D. – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2013
Learning to read in any language requires learning to map among print, sound and meaning. Writing systems differ in a number of factors that influence both the ease and the rate with which reading skill can be acquired, as well as the eventual division of labor between phonological and semantic processes. Further, developmental reading disability…
Descriptors: Reading Skills, Semantics, Reading Difficulties, Chinese
Jones, Peter E. – Language and Education, 2013
This paper examines the key linguistic arguments underpinning Basil Bernstein's theory of "elaborated" and "restricted" "codes". Building on a review of selected highlights from the collective critical response to Bernstein, the paper attempts to clarify the relationship of the theory to "deficit" views…
Descriptors: Linguistic Theory, Cognitive Processes, Models, Literacy
Di Pietro, Marie; Ptak, Radek; Schnider, Armin – Neuropsychologia, 2012
Patients with letter-by-letter alexia may have residual access to lexical or semantic representations of words despite severely impaired overt word recognition (reading). Here, we report a multilingual patient with severe letter-by-letter alexia who rapidly identified the language of written words and sentences in French and English while he had…
Descriptors: Sentences, Semantics, Written Language, Multilingualism
Law, Sam-Po; Yeung, Olivia – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2010
This study examined the effects of the age of acquisition (AOA) and semantic transparency on the reading aloud ability of a Chinese dyslexic individual, TWT, who relied on the semantic pathway to name characters. Both AOA and semantic transparency significantly predicted naming accuracy and distinguished the occurrence of correct responses and…
Descriptors: Reading Aloud to Others, Semantics, Age, Dyslexia
Andrews, Mark; Vigliocco, Gabriella; Vinson, David – Psychological Review, 2009
The authors identify 2 major types of statistical data from which semantic representations can be learned. These are denoted as "experiential data" and "distributional data". Experiential data are derived by way of experience with the physical world and comprise the sensory-motor data obtained through sense receptors. Distributional data, by…
Descriptors: Semantics, Written Language, Statistical Distributions, Statistical Data
Talking about Writing: What We Can Learn from Conversations between Parents and Their Young Children
Robins, Sarah; Treiman, Rebecca – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2009
In six analyses using the Child Language Data Exchange System known as CHILDES, we explored whether and how parents and their 1.5- to 5-year-old children talk about writing. Parent speech might include information about the similarity between print and speech and about the difference between writing and drawing. Parents could convey similarity…
Descriptors: Semantics, Written Language, Freehand Drawing, Linguistic Input
Toyoda, Etsuko – Reading Matrix: An International Online Journal, 2007
The paper reviewed studies in word-level processing skills and related areas, and profiled how the development of L2 word recognition and integration skills would contribute to autonomous "kango" (Chinese originated words or words created from Chinese originated words) vocabulary learning. Despite the fact that the acquisition of a…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, Vocabulary Development, Web Sites, Dictionaries
Wu, Xiaoying; Anderson, Richard C. – Literacy Teaching and Learning, 2007
The purpose of this study was to examine the character identification strategies of Chinese children during their oral reading of a continuous text. Eighteen second graders' oral reading of a story, as well as an interview about their decoding strategies, were audiotaped and transcribed. The results generally converged with those of previous oral…
Descriptors: Reading Research, Oral Reading, Metalinguistics, Written Language

Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine; Downey, Doris M. – Volta Review, 1992
The Colorado Process Analysis of the Written Language of Hearing-Impaired Children assesses the semantic characteristics of expressive written language narratives prepared by beginning writers. The validity of the instrument was examined with 284 children (ages 7-21), indicating that the model does identify characteristics that are critical to the…
Descriptors: Error Analysis (Language), Hearing Impairments, Semantics, Story Grammar

Goodman, Yetta – Educational Horizons, 1985
The author shares her insights about the principles and knowledge of the writing system that children discover, develop, and learn to control. She categorizes these principles as functional principles, linguistic principles, and relational principles. (CT)
Descriptors: Child Development, Language Usage, Self Expression, Semantics
Christensen, Carol A. – British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2004
Background: Orthographic-motor integration refers to the way in which orthographic knowledge is integrated with fine-motor demands of handwriting. A strong relationship has shown to exist between orthographic-motor integration and students' ability to produce creative and well-structured written text (De La Paz & Graham, 1995). This…
Descriptors: Grade 8, Written Language, Semantics, Intervention
Brice, Roanne G. – Intervention in School and Clinic, 2004
Written language requires prior knowledge of many foundation language skills. Students with language learning disabilities find it difficult to integrate language skills into academic writing assignments. Exceptional educators can teach foundation writing skills through certain underlying components of language, that is, phonology, morphology,…
Descriptors: Prior Learning, Written Language, Writing Skills, Syntax
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2