Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Patients | 10 |
Visual Perception | 10 |
Brain | 4 |
Neurological Impairments | 4 |
Adults | 2 |
Alzheimers Disease | 2 |
Attention | 2 |
Behavior | 2 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Classification | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Brain and Cognition | 3 |
Neuropsychologia | 3 |
Developmental Science | 1 |
Psychological Assessment | 1 |
Psychological Review | 1 |
Research in Autism Spectrum… | 1 |
Author
Danckert, James | 2 |
Alescio-Lautier, B. | 1 |
Allen, Harriet | 1 |
Anzures, Gizelle | 1 |
Cavezian, Celine | 1 |
Chambon, C. | 1 |
Dalery, Jean | 1 |
Deco, Gustavo | 1 |
Elahmadi, A. | 1 |
Fujita, Takako | 1 |
Griffin, G. A. Elmer | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Evaluative | 10 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Robbins, Rachel A.; Maurer, Daphne; Hatry, Alexandra; Anzures, Gizelle; Mondloch, Catherine J. – Developmental Science, 2012
We used opposing figural aftereffects to investigate whether there are at least partially separable representations of upright and inverted faces in patients who missed early visual experience because of bilateral congenital cataracts (mean age at test 19.5 years). Visually normal adults and 10-year-olds were tested for comparison. Adults showed…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Patients, Investigations, Adults
Mavritsaki, Eirini; Heinke, Dietmar; Allen, Harriet; Deco, Gustavo; Humphreys, Glyn W. – Psychological Review, 2011
We present the case for a role of biologically plausible neural network modeling in bridging the gap between physiology and behavior. We argue that spiking-level networks can allow "vertical" translation between physiological properties of neural systems and emergent "whole-system" performance--enabling psychological results to be simulated from…
Descriptors: Attention, Visual Perception, Physiology, Behavior
Skakoon-Sparling, Shayna P.; Vasquez, Brandon P.; Hano, Kate; Danckert, James – Brain and Cognition, 2011
The superior parietal cortex is critical for the control of visually guided actions. Research suggests that visual stimuli relevant to actions are preferentially processed when they are in peripersonal space. One recent study demonstrated that visually guided movements towards the body were more impaired in a patient with damage to superior…
Descriptors: Brain, Neurological Organization, Neurological Impairments, Visual Perception
Parvocellular Pathway Impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Evidence from Visual Evoked Potentials
Fujita, Takako; Yamasaki, Takao; Kamio, Yoko; Hirose, Shinichi; Tobimatsu, Shozo – Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2011
In humans, visual information is processed via parallel channels: the parvocellular (P) pathway analyzes color and form information, whereas the magnocellular (M) stream plays an important role in motion analysis. Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often show superior performance in processing fine detail, but impaired performance in…
Descriptors: Autism, Motion, Patients, Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Kim, Nam-Gyoon; Park, Jong-Hee – Neuropsychologia, 2010
Recent research has demonstrated that Alzheimer's disease (AD) affects the visual sensory pathways, producing a variety of visual deficits, including the capacity to perceive structure-from-motion (SFM). Because the sensory areas of the adult brain are known to retain a large degree of plasticity, the present study was conducted to explore whether…
Descriptors: Alzheimers Disease, Motion, Patients, Memory
Mayor-Dubois, C.; Maeder, P.; Zesiger, P.; Roulet-Perez, E. – Neuropsychologia, 2010
We investigated procedural learning in 18 children with basal ganglia (BG) lesions or dysfunctions of various aetiologies, using a visuo-motor learning test, the Serial Reaction Time (SRT) task, and a cognitive learning test, the Probabilistic Classification Learning (PCL) task. We compared patients with early (less than 1 year old, n=9), later…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Neurological Impairments, Pathology, Patients
Cavezian, Celine; Danckert, James; Lerond, Jerome; Dalery, Jean; d'Amato, Thierry; Saoud, Mohamed – Brain and Cognition, 2007
Previous studies have suggested a right hemineglect in schizophrenia, however few assessed possible visual-perceptual implication in this lateralized anomaly. A manual line bisection without (i.e., lines presented on their own) or with a local cueing paradigm (i.e., a number placed at one or both ends of the line) and the Motor-free Visual…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Schizophrenia, Depression (Psychology), Patients
Alescio-Lautier, B.; Michel, B. F.; Herrera, C.; Elahmadi, A.; Chambon, C.; Touzet, C.; Paban, V. – Neuropsychologia, 2007
It has been proposed that visual recognition memory and certain attentional mechanisms are impaired early in Alzheimer disease (AD). Little is known about visuospatial recognition memory in AD. The crucial role of the hippocampus on spatial memory and its damage in AD suggest that visuospatial recognition memory may also be impaired early. The aim…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability, Attention
Young, Garry – Brain and Cognition, 2006
This paper questions whether affordances are allied exclusively to dorsal stream processing within the visual system, or whether in fact different affordances are subserved by functionally independent neural pathways. Using case study evidence from patients with various visual pathologies, I argue that affordances can be categorised into type…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Pathology, Case Studies, Neurology

Griffin, G. A. Elmer; And Others – Psychological Assessment, 1996
A new qualitative scoring system for the Rey Visual Memory Test was tested for its ability to distinguish between malingerers and nonmalingerers. The new system, based on the types of errors made, was able to distinguish between 53 psychiatrically disabled and 64 normal nonmalingerers, and between nonmalingerers and 91 possible malingerers. (SLD)
Descriptors: Adults, Identification, Memory, Mental Disorders