NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 11 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Obersteiner, Andreas; Reiss, Kristina; Ufer, Stefan – Learning and Instruction, 2013
Theories of psychology and mathematics education recommend two instructional approaches to develop students' mental representations of number: The "exact" approach focuses on the development of exact representations of organized dot patterns; the "approximate" approach focuses on the approximate representation of analogue magnitudes. This study…
Descriptors: Numbers, Arithmetic, Mathematics Skills, Grade 1
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Durkin, Kelley; Rittle-Johnson, Bethany – Learning and Instruction, 2012
Comparing common mathematical errors to correct examples may facilitate learning, even for students with limited prior domain knowledge. We examined whether studying incorrect and correct examples was more effective than studying two correct examples across prior knowledge levels. Fourth- and fifth-grade students (N = 74) learned about decimal…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Arithmetic, Models, Instructional Materials
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van Galen, Mirte S.; Reitsma, Pieter – Learning and Instruction, 2010
The acquisition of addition facts was investigated in a practice study. Participants were 103 Grade 1 children who practiced simple addition problems with three different methods: (a) writing down the answer, (b) choosing between two alternative answers, and (c) filling in the second missing addend. On a test with simple addition problems,…
Descriptors: Grade 1, Arithmetic, Mathematics Instruction, Problem Solving
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
De Smedt, Bert; Torbeyns, Joke; Stassens, Nick; Ghesquiere, Pol; Verschaffel, Lieven – Learning and Instruction, 2010
This study examined the development of indirect addition as an alternative for solving multidigit subtractions, by means of two learning environments in traditionally schooled third-graders in Flanders (Belgium). Thirty-five third-graders, who did not demonstrate mastery of indirect addition, participated in an Explicit (n = 20) or Implicit (n =…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Educational Environment, Grade 3, Arithmetic
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Viljaranta, Jaana; Lerkkanen, Marja-Kristiina; Poikkeus, Anna-Maija; Aunola, Kaisa; Nurmi, Jari-Erik – Learning and Instruction, 2009
To examine the cross-lagged relationships between children's task motivation in mathematics and literacy, and their related performance, 139 children aged 5-6 years were examined twice during their kindergarten year. The results showed that only math-related task motivation and arithmetic performance showed cross-lagged relationship: the higher…
Descriptors: Student Motivation, Kindergarten, Arithmetic, Literacy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gamo, Sylvie; Sander, Emmanuel; Richard, Jean-Francois – Learning and Instruction, 2010
Transfer of strategies between problems sharing the same formal structure is facilitated by a semantic recoding that makes evident the structural similarities between the problems. Two experiments were carried out among 4th and 5th grade pupils, with an experimental group trained to compare strategies in order to reinterpret an arithmetic word…
Descriptors: Experimental Groups, Control Groups, Semantics, Word Problems (Mathematics)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Krajewski, Kristin; Schneider, Wolfgang – Learning and Instruction, 2009
This article reports results of a four-year longitudinal study that investigated the impact of specific and non-specific precursors on mathematical school achievement. Preschool quantity-number competencies (QNC) predicted mathematical achievement in primary school. Furthermore, basic arithmetic fact retrieval in Grade 1 had an impact on early…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Status, Arithmetic, Nonverbal Ability, Kindergarten
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Cauzinille-Marmeche, Evelyne; Julo, Jean – Learning and Instruction, 1998
This study focused on the extent to which the comparison of isomorphic problems might enrich subjects' representations and solving strategies. Results with 166 sixth graders solving isomorphic problems show that comprehension increased as problems were processed and that simultaneous presentation of all problems was more favorable than successive…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Comparative Analysis, Genetics, Grade 6
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Torbeyns, Joke; Verschaffel, Lieven; Ghesquiere, Pol – Learning and Instruction, 2004
This study investigated ability-related differences in strategy use and development in the domain of simple arithmetic, in terms of the model of strategic change, using the choice/no-choice method and the chronological-age/ability-level-match design. Twenty-six second-graders with strong mathematical abilities (MA), 25 second-graders with weak MA,…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Mathematical Aptitude, Learning Strategies, Grade 2
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Beishuizen, M.; And Others – Learning and Instruction, 1997
Mental arithmetic strategies were studied with 91 Dutch third graders who computed by splitting off 10s and units in both numbers or counting by 10s up or down from the first unsplit number. Results reveal flexibility in changing between and within strategy use. Implications for instruction are discussed. (SLD)
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hennessy, Sara – Learning and Instruction, 1993
This article describes an empirical investigation of the extent to which incorrect arithmetic algorithms persist over time. Results with 30 fourth-year and third-year English students over up to 3 months shed light on "bugs," or students' learning of incorrect concepts, and indicate that they are not very stable in children of this age.…
Descriptors: Algorithms, Arithmetic, Concept Formation, Elementary Education