Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Language Patterns | 12 |
Suffixes | 12 |
Morphology (Languages) | 6 |
Language Research | 4 |
Nouns | 4 |
Adjectives | 3 |
Grammar | 3 |
Morphophonemics | 3 |
Phonology | 3 |
Syntax | 3 |
Child Language | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Language Sciences | 2 |
Annals of Dyslexia | 1 |
Francais dans le Monde | 1 |
Glossa | 1 |
Journal of Child Language | 1 |
Journal of Psycholinguistic… | 1 |
Language | 1 |
Language and Cognitive… | 1 |
Language in Society | 1 |
Meta | 1 |
Author
Barker, Chris | 1 |
Bates, Elizabeth | 1 |
Borys, Robert H. | 1 |
Cannon, Garland | 1 |
Carlisle, Joanne F. | 1 |
Chung, Wei-Lun | 1 |
Dank, Maya | 1 |
Dench, Alan | 1 |
Deutsch, Avital | 1 |
Ibrahim, Amr Helmy | 1 |
Jarmulowicz, Linda | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 12 |
Journal Articles | 11 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 1 |
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Australia | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Chung, Wei-Lun; Jarmulowicz, Linda – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2017
For monolingual English-speaking children, judgment and production of stress in derived words, including words with phonologically neutral (e.g., -ness) and non-neutral suffixes (e.g., "-ity"), is important to both academic vocabulary growth and to word reading. For Mandarin-speaking adult English learners (AELs) the challenge of…
Descriptors: Mandarin Chinese, Native Speakers, Suprasegmentals, Second Language Learning
Deutsch, Avital; Dank, Maya – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2011
A common characteristic of subject-predicate agreement errors (usually termed attraction errors) in complex noun phrases is an asymmetrical pattern of error distribution, depending on the inflectional state of the nouns comprising the complex noun phrase. That is, attraction is most likely to occur when the head noun is the morphologically…
Descriptors: Semitic Languages, Language Patterns, Nouns, Suffixes
Trevian, Ives – Language Sciences, 2007
The present study is an attempt to account for current changes taking place in the behaviour of what are commonly taken to be stress-neutral endings in contemporary British English. The methodological framework being that of Lionel Guierre, this study aims for comprehensive coverage, via a survey of Guierre's original database (which was initially…
Descriptors: Language Patterns, Suffixes, Dictionaries, English

Kuczaj, Stan A., II; Borys, Robert H. – Language Sciences, 1988
Three- to nine-year-olds' (N=80) post-exposure production of regular and irregular suffixes indicated that subjects found it easier to learn a regular suffix when they heard it used with phonetically similar base forms. Subjects were more likely to overgeneralize the regular suffix to irregular forms when they had heard it used in conjunction with…
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Patterns, Language Processing, Morphophonemics

Barker, Chris – Language, 1998
Offers a detailed analysis of the English suffix "-ee" (employee, refugee, etc.) based on 1,500 naturally occurring tokens of 500 word types. Argues that certain semantic constraints, taken together, amount to a special-purpose thematic role that actively constrains productive use of derivational morphology. (Author/MSE)
Descriptors: English, Language Patterns, Language Research, Linguistic Theory

Wheeler, Cathy J.; Schumsky, Donald, A. – Glossa, 1980
The results of three experiments investigating where native speakers have a morpheme boundary between stems and word-final English derivational suffixes are reported. The way speakers organize phonological data is demonstrated. The results challenge the generative phonological hypothesis of maximal generalization and assumptions concerning…
Descriptors: Generative Phonology, Language Patterns, Language Research, Morphology (Languages)
Ibrahim, Amr Helmy – Francais dans le Monde, 1986
The success of the suffixes "-tique" and "-ciel" in invading the French language in the form of neologisms is examined. An interview with a specialist in linguistics and information sciences concerning the development and usage of these suffixes is included. (MSE)
Descriptors: Diachronic Linguistics, Etymology, French, Language Patterns

Carlisle, Joanne F. – Annals of Dyslexia, 1987
Normal students (N=65) in fourth, sixth, and eighth grades were compared to 17 learning-disabled ninth graders on learning derivational morphology and spelling derived forms. Disabled students' knowledge of derivational morphology was equivalent to that of normal sixth graders, but spelling of derived forms was equivalent to that of fourth…
Descriptors: Intermediate Grades, Junior High Schools, Language Patterns, Learning Disabilities

Bates, Elizabeth; Rankin, Jane – Journal of Child Language, 1979
Reports on research on the acquisition of adjectives vs inflectional endings in Italian children. Patterns resulting from a longitudinal study involving two children and an experiment involving 84 children are compared to patterns of adults participating in the latter experiment. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Adults, Child Language, Grammar
Cannon, Garland – Meta, 1979
Examines the syntactic aspects of affixation and compounding, processes accounting for the great majority of new word formations documented in "6,000 Words" (1976). (AM)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Conference Reports, Form Classes (Languages), Language Patterns
Wilson, Perkins – 1978
This guide presents a system for determining the qender of French nouns that has been tested against 12,000 pages of general reading: novels, history, social science, law, finance, and the Bible. The guide discusses the rules for determining gender and also presents various lists of the non-routine or exceptions to the rules. It is asserted that…
Descriptors: Adjectives, Determiners (Languages), Form Classes (Languages), French

Dench, Alan – Language in Society, 1987
Describes the functions of a verbal derivational suffix found in the Ngayarda languages of Western Australia. This suffix has a general "collective activity" meaning, but may be used to indicate the existence of a particular kin relationship between participants involved in the action described. (Author/LMO)
Descriptors: Anthropological Linguistics, Australian Aboriginal Languages, Kinship, Kinship Terminology