NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Type
Reports - Research57
Journal Articles48
Speeches/Meeting Papers3
Information Analyses1
Audience
Practitioners1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 57 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Emanuel Bylund; Steven Samuel; Panos Athanasopoulos – Language Learning, 2024
Research has shown that speakers of different languages may differ in their cognitive and perceptual processing of reality. A common denominator of this line of investigation has been its reliance on the sensory domain of vision. The aim of our study was to extend the scope to a new sense-taste. Using as a starting point crosslinguistic…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Language Usage, Classification, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Richtsmeier, Peter T.; Good, Amanda K. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2018
Purpose: Frequent sounds and frequent words are both acquired at an earlier age and are produced by children more accurately. Recent research suggests that frequency is not always a facilitative concept, however. Interactions between input frequency in perception and practice frequency in production may limit or inhibit growth. In this study, we…
Descriptors: Child Language, Oral Language, Young Children, Vocabulary Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Davis, E. Emory; Landau, Barbara – Language Learning and Development, 2021
Perception verbs and mental verbs have significant overlap in their syntax and semantics; both reference mental representations when taking embedded clauses, as in "I see that Maria was here" and "I think that Maria was here." Some have suggested that perception is more accessible for young children than mental states, raising…
Descriptors: Verbs, Semantics, Phrase Structure, Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van Krieken, Kobie – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2018
Narratives frequently represent perceptions that allow for multiple interpretations in terms of perspective: Perceptions can be interpreted from the narrator's viewpoint as well as the character's viewpoint. Two experiments examined the role of contextual viewpoint markers and verb tense in readers' interpretation of such ambiguous perceptions.…
Descriptors: Verbs, Grammar, Morphemes, Narration
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Perea, Manuel; Jiménez, María; Gomez, Pablo – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
In the quest to unveil the nature of the orthographic code, a useful strategy is to examine the transposed-letter effect (e.g., JUGDE is more confusable with its base word, JUDGE, than the replacement-letter nonword JUPTE). A leading explanation of this phenomenon, which is in line with models of visual attention, is that there is perceptual…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Alphabets, Coding, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ostarek, Markus; Vigliocco, Gabriella – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Previous research has shown that processing words with an up/down association (e.g., bird, foot) can influence the subsequent identification of visual targets in congruent location (at the top/bottom of the screen). However, as facilitation and interference were found under similar conditions, the nature of the underlying mechanisms remained…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Association (Psychology), Perception, Simulation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Heyselaar, Evelien; Wheeldon, Linda; Segaert, Katrien – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
Structural priming is the tendency to repeat syntactic structure across sentences and can be divided into short-term (prime to immediately following target) and long-term (across an experimental session) components. This study investigates how nondeclarative memory could support both the transient, short-term and the persistent, long-term…
Descriptors: Priming, Memory, Short Term Memory, Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hakvoort, Britt; de Bree, Elise; van der Leij, Aryan; Maassen, Ben; van Setten, Ellie; Maurits, Natasha; van Zuijen, Titia L. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2016
Purpose: This study assessed whether a categorical speech perception (CP) deficit is associated with dyslexia or familial risk for dyslexia, by exploring a possible cascading relation from speech perception to phonology to reading and by identifying whether speech perception distinguishes familial risk (FR) children with dyslexia (FRD) from those…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Speech, Perception, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Truscott, John – Second Language Research, 2015
Understanding the place of consciousness in second language acquisition (SLA) is crucial for an understanding of how acquisition occurs. Considerable work has been done on this topic, but nearly all of it assumes a highly non-modular view, according to which language and its development is "nothing special". As this assumption runs…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, Language Research, Guidelines, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McCarthy, Kathleen M.; Mahon, Merle; Rosen, Stuart; Evans, Bronwen G. – Child Development, 2014
The majority of bilingual speech research has focused on simultaneous bilinguals. Yet, in immigrant communities, children are often initially exposed to their family language (L1), before becoming gradually immersed in the host country's language (L2). This is typically referred to as sequential bilingualism. Using a longitudinal design, this…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Longitudinal Studies, Speech, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McCauley, Stewart M; Hestvik, Arild; Vogel, Irene – Language and Speech, 2013
Previous research using picture/word matching tasks has demonstrated a tendency to incorrectly interpret phrasally stressed strings as compounds. Using event-related potentials, we sought to determine whether this pattern stems from poor perceptual sensitivity to the compound/phrasal stress distinction, or from a post-perceptual bias in behavioral…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Suprasegmentals, Brain, Cognitive Measurement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ellis Weismer, Susan; Haebig, Eileen; Edwards, Jan; Saffran, Jenny; Venker, Courtney E. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2016
This study investigated whether vocabulary delays in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) can be explained by a cognitive style that prioritizes processing of detailed, local features of input over global contextual integration--as claimed by the weak central coherence (WCC) theory. Thirty toddlers with ASD and 30 younger,…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Vocabulary Development, Toddlers, Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Leclercq, Anne-Lise; Maillart, Christelle; Majerus, Steve – Topics in Language Disorders, 2013
Children with specific language impairment (SLI) consistently show poor nonword repetition (NWR) performance. However, the reason for these difficulties remains a matter of intensive debate. Nonword repetition is a complex psycholinguistic task that heavily relies upon phonological segmentation and phonological knowledge, and even lexical…
Descriptors: Speech Impairments, Language Impairments, Repetition, Psycholinguistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kharlamov, Viktor; Campbell, Kenneth; Kazanina, Nina – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Speech sounds are not always perceived in accordance with their acoustic-phonetic content. For example, an early and automatic process of perceptual repair, which ensures conformity of speech inputs to the listener's native language phonology, applies to individual input segments that do not exist in the native inventory or to sound sequences that…
Descriptors: Phonology, Speech, Perception, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ratcliff, Roger; Love, Jessica; Thompson, Clarissa A.; Opfer, John E. – Child Development, 2012
Children (n = 130; M[subscript age] = 8.51-15.68 years) and college-aged adults (n = 72; M[subscript age] = 20.50 years) completed numerosity discrimination and lexical decision tasks. Children produced longer response times (RTs) than adults. R. Ratcliff's (1978) diffusion model, which divides processing into components (e.g., quality of…
Descriptors: Children, Young Adults, Older Adults, Reaction Time
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4