Descriptor
Linguistic Performance | 20 |
Sentence Structure | 20 |
Child Language | 14 |
Language Acquisition | 14 |
Language Research | 11 |
Psycholinguistics | 11 |
Syntax | 8 |
Preschool Children | 7 |
Verbal Development | 7 |
Semantics | 6 |
Verbs | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Child Language | 3 |
Applied Psycholinguistics | 1 |
Brain and Language | 1 |
CLE Working Papers | 1 |
Cognition | 1 |
Journal of Memory and Language | 1 |
Journal of Phonetics | 1 |
Language Learning | 1 |
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly… | 1 |
Author
Aaronson, Doris | 1 |
Bowerman, Melissa | 1 |
Carrell, Patricia L. | 1 |
Chapman, Robin S. | 1 |
Clahsen, Harald | 1 |
Clumeck, Harold | 1 |
Cooper, William E., Ed. | 1 |
Cutler, Anne | 1 |
Dent, Cathy H. | 1 |
Ferres, Steven | 1 |
Fodor, Jerry A. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 20 |
Journal Articles | 10 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Wenzlaff, Michaela; Clahsen, Harald – Brain and Language, 2004
This study presents results from sentence-completion and grammaticality-judgment tasks with 7 German-speaking agrammatic aphasics and 7 age-matched control subjects examining tense and subject-verb agreement marking. For both experimental tasks, we found that the aphasics achieved high correctness scores for agreement, while tense marking was…
Descriptors: Grammar, German, Aphasia, Morphemes
Stevenson, Patrick – CLE Working Papers, 1994
This paper examines the claim that German-language syntax is undergoing a process of restructuring that will eliminate verb final position in sentences, resulting in a very English-style linear sentence structure. One particular structure is examined in interviews with 30 adults and 10 children: the finite verb in subordinate clauses that is…
Descriptors: German, Grammatical Acceptability, Interviews, Language Patterns

Maratsos, Michael P.; Kuczaj, Stanley A., II – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (under the title "What a Child Can Do Before He Will"), 1974
A study was undertaken to determine how much knowledge children have of grammatical systems before they evidence the systems in their spontaneous speech in a productive way. A child aged about two and a half years was examined over several months through elicited imitation causing him to repeat a model sentence immediately after the researcher.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Grammar, Imitation, Language Acquisition

Cutler, Anne; Fodor, Jerry A. – Cognition, 1979
Reaction time to detect a phoneme target in a sentence was faster when the target-containing word formed part of the semantic focus of the sentence. Sentence understanding was facilitated by rapid identification of focused information. Active search for accented words can be interpreted as a search for semantic focus. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Adults, Higher Education, Linguistic Performance, Listening Comprehension

Richards, Meredith Martin – Journal of Child Language, 1979
Ninety children between the ages of three and six described objects which differed on three simultaneous dimensions, using adjective combinations appropriate to the dimensions. Each child performed an imitation, comprehension, and production task. (Author/CFM)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Research

Schuckers, Gordon H.; Lefkov, Carol S. – Journal of Phonetics, 1979
Twenty-four normal, misarticulation-free second-grade children participated in tasks designed to test their ability to perceive misarticulations in contextual speech. Results indicate that children are able to successfully identify sentences in which misarticulated words occur in addition to specific misarticulated words within sentences. (SW)
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Child Language, Elementary School Students, Error Analysis (Language)
Scollon, Ronald – 1973
Previous studies have defined the earliest stage of child language to be the stage at which an uninitiated speaker of adult language can understand sentences spoken by the child. Upon the examination of the language of one child, aged 1 year and 7 months, it became evident that she could talk, even though it was equally evident that she didn't use…
Descriptors: Child Language, Context Clues, Distinctive Features (Language), Language Acquisition

Aaronson, Doris; Ferres, Steven – Journal of Memory and Language, 1986
Compares the processing of English words by Chinese-English bilinguals with that of monolingual English speakers. Subjects read and rated English words for their contribution to sentence structure and meaning. It was found that bilinguals generally rated English words as contributing more to sentence structure and meaning than did monolinguals.…
Descriptors: Bilingual Students, Chinese Americans, College Students, Contrastive Linguistics
Roeper, Thomas; Mattei, Edward – 1974
Comprehension of the quantifiers "some" and "all" was studied with 202 children, three to nine years old. Thirty-two quantifier sentences dealing with descriptions of circles and squares were presented to the children. Wooden objects were presented to some children to see if results were affected by the choice of abstract objects, but no…
Descriptors: Child Language, Children, Comprehension, Deep Structure

Greenfield, Patricia M.; Dent, Cathy H. – Journal of Child Language, 1982
Contrasts the syntactic view of forward and backward deletion of base structure elements with the idea that pragmatic factors of situational redundancy and perceptual grouping account for conjunction reduction in children. (EKN)
Descriptors: Child Language, Children, Conjunctions, Deep Structure
Chapman, Robin S.; Kohn, Lawrence L. – 1977
A study was conducted to determine whether children give evidence of using any of six comprehension strategies and whether children of same and different ages use different strategies. It was studied how comprehension performance can best be predicted by other facts about the child, including his language and his language input. The six…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis, Comprehension
Hakuta, Kenji – 1977
Comprehension of reversible active and passive sentences was studied with 48 Japanese children between the ages of two and six. Four types of sentences were constructed using passive and active structures and two word orders: subject-object-verb (SOV) and object-subject-verb (OSV). The basic order of elements in a simple sentence in Japanese is…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Grammar

Harada, Kazuko I. – 1976
By age two, a child begins to form complex sentences by joining two or more sentences or by embedding one sentence into another. Formation of conjoined structures is a simpler process and emerges earlier than that of embedding structures. This paper attempts to answer the following questions: (1) Do children produce or understand embedding…
Descriptors: Child Language, Comprehension, Imitation, Intellectual Development

Smith, Carlota S.; van Kleeck, Anne – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Reports an experimental investigation of the influence of linguistic factors on linguistic performance. The factors studied were interpretive complexity and surface length. Results show an interaction between types of linguistic complexity and type of linguistic performance. (Author/SED)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Language, Difficulty Level, Error Analysis (Language)
Bowerman, Melissa – 1977
The acquisition of rules for formulating causative verbs was studied with children over a period of a few years. Most of the data is based on the spontaneous speech of the author's two daughters, from age 2;6 to 6;2 and from age 2;4 to 3;11. It was hypothesized that there are at least two prerequisites for the child's formulation of a general rule…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2