Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Control Groups | 6 |
Phonology | 6 |
Dyslexia | 3 |
Elementary School Students | 2 |
Experimental Groups | 2 |
Language Impairments | 2 |
Oral Language | 2 |
Phoneme Grapheme… | 2 |
Predictive Validity | 2 |
Preschool Children | 2 |
Tables (Data) | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Applied Psycholinguistics | 6 |
Author
Greaney, Keith T. | 1 |
Joanisse, Marc F. | 1 |
Landerl, Karin | 1 |
Montgomery, James W. | 1 |
Poulsen, Mads | 1 |
Robertson, Erin K. | 1 |
Thal, Donna J. | 1 |
Tunmer, William E. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
New Zealand | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Poulsen, Mads – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2011
Word production difficulties are well documented in dyslexia, whereas the results are mixed for receptive phonological processing. This asymmetry raises the possibility that the core phonological deficit of dyslexia is restricted to output processing stages. The present study investigated whether a group of dyslexics had word level receptive…
Descriptors: Age, Dyslexia, Word Recognition, Decision Making
Robertson, Erin K.; Joanisse, Marc F. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2010
We examined spoken sentence comprehension in school-age children with developmental dyslexia or language impairment (LI), compared to age-matched and younger controls. Sentence-picture matching tasks were employed under three different working memory (WM) loads, two levels of syntactic difficulty, and two sentence lengths. Phonological short-term…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Sentences, Syntax, Dyslexia

Montgomery, James W. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1995
Examined the phonological memory capacity, rate of articulation, phonological encoding, and perceptual-processing abilities of 13 specifically language-impaired (SLI) children and 13 language-matched normal (NL) children. The results of a nonsense word repetition task showed that SLI children repeated significantly fewer multisyllabic nonsense…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis, Control Groups

Landerl, Karin; And Others – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1996
Focuses on the importance of phonology in establishing orthographic representations. In normal readers, phonological and orthographic representations of words are so closely connected that they are usually coactivated, whereas in dyslexics, this connection is less strong, so that orthographic representations interfere less with phonemic…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Dyslexia, Error Patterns, Orthographic Symbols

Greaney, Keith T.; Tunmer, William E. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1996
Studied the relationship between the ease with which children use orthographic analogies and their reading skills. Results of an experiment using a reading age match design showed that poor readers performed as well as normal readers on orally presented measures of onset/rhyme sensitivity, but less well on visually/orally presented rhyme tasks.…
Descriptors: Analysis of Variance, Context Clues, Control Groups, Elementary School Students

Thal, Donna J.; And Others – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1995
Toddlers in the lowest 10th percentile for lexical production were compared with age- and language-matched controls on measures of phonetic complexity, lexical development, and grammatical complexity. Results indicate an overlap between phonology, lexicon, and grammar and suggest the importance of true consonant production for lexical development.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Consonants, Control Groups, Data Analysis