NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yang, J.; Cao, Z.; Xu, X.; Chen, G. – Brain and Cognition, 2012
The object of this study was to investigate whether the amygdala is involved in affective priming effect after stimuli are encoded unconsciously and consciously. During the encoding phase, each masked face (fearful or neutral) was presented to participants six times for 17 ms each, using a backward masking paradigm. During the retrieval phase,…
Descriptors: Priming, Infants, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Fear
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vervloed, Mathijs P. J.; Hendriks, Angelique W.; van den Eijnde, Esther – Brain and Cognition, 2011
Face processing development is negatively affected when infants have not been exposed to faces for some time because of congenital cataract blocking all vision (Le Grand, Mondloch, Maurer, & Brent, 2001). It is not clear, however, whether more subtle differences in face exposure may also have an influence. The present study looked at the effect of…
Descriptors: Mothers, Infants, Lateral Dominance, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gullick, Margaret M.; Temple, Elise – Brain and Cognition, 2011
While numbers generally cue processing of quantity or order, they can also contain semantic information, as in the case of historic years (e.g., "1492" calls forth associations of Columbus sailing the ocean blue). Whether these dates are processed as quantities or events may depend on the context in which they occur. We examined such "ambiguous…
Descriptors: Semantics, Numbers, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morasch, Katherine C.; Bell, Martha Ann – Brain and Cognition, 2009
This study of infant declarative memory concurrently examined brain-electrical activity and deferred imitation performance in 10-month-old infants. Continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) measures were collected throughout the activity-matched baseline, encoding (modeling) and retrieval (delayed test) phases of a within-subjects deferred imitation…
Descriptors: Imitation, Infants, Memory, Diagnostic Tests
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huggenberger, Harriet J.; Suter, Susanne E.; Reijnen, Ester; Schachinger, Hartmut – Brain and Cognition, 2009
Women's cradling side preference has been related to contralateral hemispheric specialization of processing emotional signals; but not of processing baby's facial expression. Therefore, 46 nulliparous female volunteers were characterized as left or non-left holders (HG) during a doll holding task. During a signal detection task they were then…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Nonverbal Communication, Females, Response Style (Tests)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Grossmann, Tobias; Striano, Tricia; Friederici, Angela D. – Brain and Cognition, 2007
Event-related brain potentials were measured in 7- and 12-month-old infants to examine the development of processing happy and angry facial expressions. In 7-month-olds a larger negativity to happy faces was observed at frontal, central, temporal and parietal sites (Experiment 1), whereas 12-month-olds showed a larger negativity to angry faces at…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Cognitive Processes, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Santesso, Diane L.; Schmidt, Louis A.; Trainor, Laurel J. – Brain and Cognition, 2007
Many studies have shown that infants prefer infant-directed (ID) speech to adult-directed (AD) speech. ID speech functions to aid language learning, obtain and/or maintain an infant's attention, and create emotional communication between the infant and caregiver. We examined psychophysiological responses to ID speech that varied in affective…
Descriptors: Metabolism, Visual Stimuli, Medicine, Intimacy