NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 102 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Butzin, Clifford A.; Dozier, Mary – Child Development, 1986
Three experiments investigated (1) whether developmental differences in the information integration rule apply to ulterior motive information; (2) whether such developmental differences are limited to situations involving parental reward; and (3) how related age differences among children can best be explained. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fabricius, William V.; And Others – Child Development, 1987
Assessed 3- to 7-year-old children's sensitivity to logical necessity by contrasting performance in insufficient and sufficient information conditions. A search task used in Experiments 1 and 2 allowed children to search for additional information in insufficient conditions. A judgement condition used in Experiment 2 required a "can't tell"…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Inferences, Logical Thinking, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Marsh, R. W. – Child Development, 1985
Epstein (1974) claims evidence for regular two-year growth spurts in the development of brain and mind, a phenomenon he calls phrenoblysis. Unfortunately, repeated analysis of the data he presents as proof of his theory provides no support. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Children, Cognitive Development, Data Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Feshbach, Norma Deitch; Feshbach, Seymour – Child Development, 1987
Data indicate that for girls, affective dispositional factors (empathy, depressive affectivity, aggression, and self-concept) are intimately linked to cognitive development and academic achievement. (PCB)
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Affective Behavior, Cognitive Development, Preadolescents
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gelman, Susan A.; Markman, Ellen M. – Child Development, 1987
Studies children's inductive inferences in order to investigate the development of the expectation that members of a category share unforeseen properties. Results indicate that preschoolers drew more inferences based on category membership than on perceptual appearances. (PCB)
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Induction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Craton, Lincoln G.; Yonas, Albert – Child Development, 1988
A sample of 44 infants of five months of age showed a significant reaching preference for the apparently nearer region of a computer-generated display. This indicated that the infants were sensitive to boundary flow information for depth at an edge. (RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Depth Perception, Infants, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Goodman, Gail S.; Haith, Marshall M. – Child Development, 1987
Maintains that Teyler and Fountain's presentation (1987) contains several limitations, namely, that the authors do not (1) distinguish between learning and memory, nor between storage and retrieval; (2) address the role of knowledge-based influences in memory and learning; or (3) employ concepts that can accommodate developmental phenomena in the…
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development, Learning Theories
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Inagaki, Kayoko; Hatano, Giyoo – Child Development, 1987
Results of two experiments on kindergarten children in Japan indicate that young children can, and often do, apply personification as an analogy to animate objects to generate a reasonable prediction. It was also found that children try to constrain the personification by using additional knowledge. (PCB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sodian, Beate; Wimmer, Heinz – Child Development, 1987
Four experiments studied 4- to 6-year-old children's understanding of inferential reasoning as a source of knowledge. To assess understanding that knowledge of relevant premises leads to knowledge of the conclusion, children had to judge the knowledge of another person, who was presented to the child as being aware of two premises. (Author/BN)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Inferences, Metacognition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Haith, Marshall M.; And Others – Child Development, 1988
Findings indicate that infants can detect regularity in spatiotemporal series; will develop expectancies for events in the series; and will act on the basis of those expectancies even when their actions have no effect on the stimulus events. (PCB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Expectation, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Heyns, Barbara – Child Development, 1987
Reviews the literature on the effects of summer programs, and offers a reinterpretation of the role of summer learning in cognitive development. (PCB)
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Cognitive Development, Compensatory Education, Elementary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Miller, Patricia H.; And Others – Child Development, 1986
A developmental progression in 6-, 8-, and 10-year-old children's use of strategies for gathering information was revealed in a study involving partial recall, total recall, and similarity/difference judgments. When subjects chose stimuli for exposure from an array, older children showed more ability to match strategy to task demands. Strategy…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Vaughn, Brian E.; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Delay/response inhibition in the presence of an attractive stimulus and compliance with maternal directives in a clean-up task were observed among subjects 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Results suggested (1) achievement of self-control is a major developmental accomplishment, and (2) individual differences in self-control emerge and are…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Individual Differences, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
O'Connell, Barbara G.; Gerard, Anthony B. – Child Development, 1985
Examines extent to which knowing that certain things go together and knowing how they do are independent aspects of developing cognitive systems. Indicates that children use temporal order as organizing principle by three years of age. Suggests an earlier developmental horizon as the origin of ordering abilities. (Author/BE)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Imitation, Modeling (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
List, Judith A.; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Challenges the notion that long-term memory retrieval efficiency is a potential source of individual and developmental differences in cognitive functioning. Fourth-grade, eighth-grade, and college-aged subjects participated in a task using the Posner letter matching paradigm and were assessed with tests of verbal and spatial ability. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Adults, Children, Cognitive Development
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7