Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Language Processing | 2 |
Nouns | 2 |
Preschool Children | 2 |
Child Language | 1 |
Cues | 1 |
English | 1 |
Form Classes (Languages) | 1 |
Generalization | 1 |
Identification | 1 |
Knowledge Level | 1 |
Language Acquisition | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 2 |
Author
Cimpian, Andrei | 1 |
Diesendruck, Gil | 1 |
Graham, Susan A. | 1 |
Hall, D. Geoffrey | 1 |
Markman, Ellen M. | 1 |
Meltzer, Trent J. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Preschool Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Cimpian, Andrei; Meltzer, Trent J.; Markman, Ellen M. – Child Development, 2011
Generic sentences (e.g., "Birds lay eggs") convey generalizations about entire categories and may thus be an important source of knowledge for children. However, these sentences cannot be identified by a simple rule, requiring instead the integration of multiple cues. The present studies focused on 3- to 5-year-olds' (N = 91) use of…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Nouns, Morphology (Languages)
Diesendruck, Gil; Hall, D. Geoffrey; Graham, Susan A. – Child Development, 2006
In Study 1, English-speaking 3- and 4-year-olds heard a novel adjective used to label one of two objects and were asked for the referent of a different novel adjective. Children were more likely to select the unlabeled object if the two adjectives appeared prenominally (e.g., "a very DAXY dog") than as predicates (e.g., "a dog that is very DAXY").…
Descriptors: Pragmatics, Nouns, Form Classes (Languages), Semitic Languages