NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 61 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
S. V. Wass; C. S. Smith; F. U. Mirza; E. M. G. Greenwood; L. Goupil – Child Development, 2025
Children raised in chaotic households show affect dysregulation during later childhood. To understand why, we took day-long home recordings using microphones and autonomic monitors from 74 12-month-old infant-caregiver dyads (40% male, 60% white, data collected between 2018 and 2021). Caregivers in low-Confusion Hubbub And Order Scale (chaos)…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Family Environment, Physiology, Parent Child Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Conradt, Elisabeth; Hawes, Katheleen; Guerin, Dylan; Armstrong, David A.; Marsit, Carmen J.; Tronick, Edward; Lester, Barry M. – Child Development, 2016
This study tested whether maternal responsiveness may buffer the child to the effects of maternal depressive symptoms on DNA methylation of "NR3C1," "11ß-HSD2," and neuroendocrine functioning. DNA was derived from buccal epithelial cells and prestress cortisol was obtained from the saliva of 128 infants. Mothers with depressive…
Descriptors: Genetics, Depression (Psychology), Mothers, Symptoms (Individual Disorders)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zieber, Nicole; Kangas, Ashley; Hock, Alyson; Bhatt, Ramesh S. – Child Development, 2014
Adults recognize emotions conveyed by bodies with comparable accuracy to facial emotions. However, no prior study has explored infants' perception of body emotions. In Experiment 1, 6.5-month-olds (n = 32) preferred happy over neutral actions of actors with covered faces in upright but not inverted silent videos. In Experiment 2, infants…
Descriptors: Infants, Emotional Development, Emotional Response, Human Body
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Landa, Rebecca J.; Gross, Alden L.; Stuart, Elizabeth A.; Faherty, Ashley – Child Development, 2013
Retrospective studies indicate 2 major classes of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) onset: early and later, after a period of relatively healthy development. This prospective, longitudinal study examined social, language, and motor trajectories in 235 children with and without a sibling with autism, ages 6-36 months. Children were grouped as: ASD…
Descriptors: Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Autism, Comparative Analysis, Longitudinal Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Leve, Leslie D.; Kerr, David C. R.; Shaw, Daniel; Ge, Xiaojia; Neiderhiser, Jenae M.; Scaramella, Laura V.; Reid, John B.; Conger, Rand; Reiss, David – Child Development, 2010
To further the understanding of the effects of early experiences, 9-month-old infants were observed during a frustration task. The analytical sample was composed of 348 linked triads of participants (adoptive parents, adopted child, and birth parent[s]) from a prospective adoption study. It was hypothesized that genetic risk for externalizing…
Descriptors: Mothers, Pregnancy, Infants, Genetics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lester, Barry M.; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Quantifies behavioral periodicities or cycles during face-to-face interaction between three- and five-month-old term and preterm infants and their mothers. Compares the temporal organization of social interaction between term and preterm infants. Spectral and cross-spectral techniques showed the existence of periodicities. Differences were found…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Behavior Patterns, Comparative Analysis, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hertenstein, Matthew J.; Campos, Joseph J. – Child Development, 2004
The goal of this investigation was to study the regulatory retention effects of an adult's emotional displays on infant behavior. In Study 1, 11- and 14-month-old infants were tested in a social-referencing-like paradigm in which a 1-hr delay was imposed between the exposure trials and the test trial. In Study 2, 11-month-olds were tested in the…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Infants, Affective Behavior, Retention (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gunnar, Megan R.; Stone, Cheryl – Child Development, 1984
Mothers of 48 infants approximately 12 months old displayed either positive or neutral affect while their infants responded to pleasant, ambiguous, or aversive toys. On the first trial maternal affect had no effect; on the second trial, positive maternal affect resulted in more positive infant responses, but only for the ambiguous toy. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Infant Behavior, Infants, Mothers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Balaban, Marie T. – Child Development, 1995
While 18 5-month-old infants viewed photographic slides of faces posed in happy, neutral, or angry expressions, a brief acoustic noise burst was presented to elicit the blink component of human startle. It was found that blink size was augmented during the viewing of angry expressions and reduced during viewing of happy expressions. (MDM)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Emotional Response, Facial Expressions, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hornik, Robin; And Others – Child Development, 1987
Studied were the responses of infants to new toys presented with either positive, negative, or neutral affective displays by mothers. Responses to stimulus toys were compared with responses to free play toys. Maternal displays influenced responses only to stimulus toys. (PCB)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Infant Behavior, Infants, Mothers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bloom, Lois; Capatides, Joanne Bitetti – Child Development, 1987
Results indicated that the more frequently the children studied expressed emotion, the older the age of language achievements; and the more time spent in neutral affect, the younger the age of language achievements. (PCB)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Age Differences, Individual Development, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Stenberg, Craig R.; And Others – Child Development, 1983
Investigated whether, in a sample of 30 infants, anger could reliably be observed in facial expressions as early as seven months of age. Also considered was the influence of several variables on anger responses: infants' familiarity with the frustrator, repetition of trials, and sex of the child. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Cognitive Development, Facial Expressions, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hains, S. M. J.; Muir, D. W. – Child Development, 1996
Two experiments examined the effects of changes in adult eye direction during both televised and live contingent interaction with infants 3 to 6 months of age. Infants' smiling declined whenever adults looked away, supporting the hypothesis that infants express their cognitive appreciation of the adults' eye direction by their affective behavior.…
Descriptors: Adults, Affective Behavior, Attention, Eye Contact
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Dunham, Philip; Dunham, Frances – Child Development, 1990
Infants participated in a nonsocial contingency task immediately after a social interaction with their mothers. The amount of time mothers and infants spent in a state of vocal turn-taking predicted individual differences in infants' subsequent performance on the contingency task. (PCB)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Infant Behavior, Infants, Mothers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Frodi, Ann M.; Lamb, Michael E. – Child Development, 1980
Compares the responses of 14 child abusers and a matched group of nonabusers to videotapes of crying and smiling infants. Psychophysiological and subjective self-report measures were taken. (SS)
Descriptors: Adults, Affective Behavior, Child Abuse, Comparative Analysis
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5