Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 48 |
Author
Friedman, William J. | 2 |
Kochanska, Grazyna | 2 |
Moore, Colleen F. | 2 |
Amsel, Eric | 1 |
Benish-Weisman, Maya | 1 |
Betts, Stephanie | 1 |
Bierman, Karen Linn | 1 |
Bloom, Lois | 1 |
Brownell, Celia A. | 1 |
Burchinal, Margaret | 1 |
Cahan, Sorel | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 48 |
Reports - Research | 43 |
Opinion Papers | 3 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 5 |
Location
Australia | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Japan (Tokyo) | 1 |
Ohio | 1 |
Taiwan | 1 |
United States | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Self Description Questionnaire | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Daniel, Ella; Benish-Weisman, Maya; Sneddon, Joanne N.; Lee, Julie A. – Child Development, 2020
Little is known about how children's value priorities develop over time. This study identifies children's value priority profiles and follows their development during middle childhood. Australian children (N = 609; ages 5-12 at Time 1) reported their values over 2 years. Latent Transition Analysis indicated four profiles: Social-Focus, Self-Focus,…
Descriptors: Child Development, Values, Children, Preadolescents
Cohen-Gilbert, Julia E.; Thomas, Kathleen M. – Child Development, 2013
This study investigated the changing relation between emotion and inhibitory control during adolescence. One hundred participants between 11 and 25 years of age performed a go-nogo task in which task-relevant stimuli (letters) were presented at the center of large task-irrelevant images depicting negative, positive, or neutral scenes selected from…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Psychological Patterns, Adolescents, Young Adults
Ratcliff, Roger; Love, Jessica; Thompson, Clarissa A.; Opfer, John E. – Child Development, 2012
Children (n = 130; M[subscript age] = 8.51-15.68 years) and college-aged adults (n = 72; M[subscript age] = 20.50 years) completed numerosity discrimination and lexical decision tasks. Children produced longer response times (RTs) than adults. R. Ratcliff's (1978) diffusion model, which divides processing into components (e.g., quality of…
Descriptors: Children, Young Adults, Older Adults, Reaction Time
Li, Fangfang – Child Development, 2012
Speech productions of 40 English- and 40 Japanese-speaking children (aged 2-5) were examined and compared with the speech produced by 20 adult speakers (10 speakers per language). Participants were recorded while repeating words that began with "s" and "sh" sounds. Clear language-specific patterns in adults' speech were found,…
Descriptors: Acoustics, Speech, Oral Language, Adults
Myers, Lauren J.; Liben, Lynn S. – Child Development, 2008
The contribution of intentionality understanding to symbolic development was examined. Actors added colored dots to a map, displaying either symbolic or aesthetic intentions. In Study 1, most children (5-6 years) understood actors' intentions, but when asked which graphic would help find hidden objects, most selected the incorrect (aesthetic) one…
Descriptors: Intention, Cartography, Semiotics, Age Differences
Kalish, Charles W.; Lawson, Christopher A. – Child Development, 2008
Three experiments explored the significance of deontic properties (involving rights and obligations) in representations of social categories. Preschool-aged children (M = 4.8), young school-aged children (M = 8.2), and adults judged the centrality of behavioral, psychological, and deontic properties for both familiar (Experiments 1 and 2, Ns = 50…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Children, Adults, Social Cognition

Gunnar, Megan R.; And Others – Child Development, 1996
Examined changes in cortisol and behavioral responses in 83 infants. Found that salivary cortisol responses before and after inoculation were high at 2 months, decreased between 2 and 4 months, remained stable, then declined again between 6 and 15 months. Found some evidence that emergence of circadian rhythm in cortisol might be related to early…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Emotional Response, Individual Development, Infant Behavior

Brownell, Celia A.; Carriger, Michael Sean – Child Development, 1990
Children at 12, 18, 24, and 30 months of age were observed in same-age, same-sex dyads while they attempted to solve a simple cooperation problem. Each child was given an elicited imitation task that was used to index decentration. Although no dyad of 12 month olds could cooperate, 24 and 30 month olds could coordinate behavior quickly and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cooperation, Individual Development, Infants

Taylor, Marjorie – Child Development, 1988
Studies investigated the development of children's ability to differentiate what they see from what they know in the context of conceptual perspective taking. Two developmental levels accounted for children's performance when they were asked about a naive observer's knowledge of the identity of objects. Perspective awareness training improved…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Individual Development, Perspective Taking, Visual Stimuli

Bloom, Lois; Capatides, Joanne Bitetti – Child Development, 1987
Results indicated that the more frequently the children studied expressed emotion, the older the age of language achievements; and the more time spent in neutral affect, the younger the age of language achievements. (PCB)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Age Differences, Individual Development, Infant Behavior

O'Neill, Daniela K.; And Others – Child Development, 1992
Three studies investigated the degree to which young children understand that the acquisition of certain types of knowledge depends on the modality of the sensory experience involved. Results suggest that an appreciation of the different types of knowledge our senses can provide develops between the ages of three and five years. (GLR)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Individual Development, Learning Modalities, Metacognition

Friedman, William J. – Child Development, 1991
In this study of the distinction between temporal distance and location, children were asked to judge the relative recency and time of target events that occurred one and seven weeks before testing. All judged recency and localized time of day correctly. Six- and eight- but not four-year olds localized longer time scales. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cues, Individual Development, Memory

Schneider-Rosen, Karen; Wenz-Gross, Melodie – Child Development, 1990
Results suggest that the most adaptive course of action for children may be to be responsive to environmental demands and interpersonal constraints. In the study, the patterns of compliance of 31 children of 18 months, 32 children of 24 months, and 36 children of 30 months, and their mothers and fathers were observed in five situations. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Patterns, Compliance (Psychology), Fathers

Hale, Sandra – Child Development, 1990
Finds support for the global trend hypothesis which posits that cognitive processing speed changes as a function of age and all component processes change at the same rate. (PCB)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Child Development, Cognitive Processes

Pipp-Siegel, Sandra; Foltz, Carol – Child Development, 1997
Two studies tested whether 12- and 24-month olds' self-knowledge differed from their knowledge of others (mother or inanimate object), or whether toddlers' knowledge of persons (self and mother) differed from knowledge of objects. Results showed that 12-month olds were more sensitive than older toddlers to perceptual features of objects. Data were…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Individual Development, Mothers, Object Permanence