NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 32 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zhao, Wanlin; Li, Baike; Shanks, David R.; Zhao, Wenbo; Zheng, Jun; Hu, Xiao; Su, Ningxin; Fan, Tian; Yin, Yue; Luo, Liang; Yang, Chunliang – Child Development, 2022
Recent studies established that making concurrent judgments of learning (JOLs) can significantly alter (typically enhance) memory itself--a "reactivity" effect. The current study recruited 190 Chinese children (M[subscript age] = 8.68 years; 101 female) in 2020 and 2021 to explore the reactivity effect on children's learning, its…
Descriptors: Evaluative Thinking, Memory, Metacognition, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Breitwieser, Jasmin; Brod, Garvin – Child Development, 2021
This study examined age-related differences in the effectiveness of two generative learning strategies (GLSs). Twenty-five children aged 9-11 and 25 university students aged 17-29 performed a facts learning task in which they had to generate either a prediction or an example before seeing the correct result. We found a significant Age × Learning…
Descriptors: Learning Strategies, Preadolescents, Young Adults, College Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sobel, David M.; Letourneau, Susan M. – Child Development, 2018
It is widely believed that exploration is a mechanism for young children's learning. The present investigation examines preschoolers' beliefs about how learning occurs. We asked 3- to 5-year-olds to articulate how characters in a set of stories learned about a new toy. Younger preschoolers were more likely to overemphasize the role of characters'…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Beliefs, Inquiry, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yu, Yue; Bonawitz, Elizabeth; Shafto, Patrick – Child Development, 2019
Questioning is a core component of formal pedagogy. Parents commonly question children, but do they use questions to teach? This article defines "pedagogical questions" as questions for which the questioner already knows the answer and intended to help the questionee learn. Transcripts of parent-child conversations were collected from…
Descriptors: Parent Child Relationship, Databases, Teaching Methods, Questioning Techniques
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gruber, Thibaud; Deschenaux, Amélie; Frick, Aurélien; Clément, Fabrice – Child Development, 2019
Group membership is a strong driver of everyday life in humans, influencing similarity judgments, trust choices, and learning processes. However, its ontogenetic development remains to be understood. This study investigated how group membership, age, sex, and identification with a team influenced 39- to 60-month-old children (N = 94) in a series…
Descriptors: Group Membership, Learning Processes, Age Differences, Gender Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chudek, Maciej; Baron, Andrew S.; Birch, Susan – Child Development, 2016
Children are both shrewd about whom to copy--they selectively learn from certain adults--and overimitators--they copy adults' obviously superfluous actions. Is overimitation also selective? Does selectivity change with age? In two experiments, 161 two- to seven-year-old children saw videos of one adult receiving better payoffs or more bystander…
Descriptors: Children, Imitation, Modeling (Psychology), Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lockhart, Kristi L.; Goddu, Mariel K.; Smith, Eric D.; Keil, Frank C. – Child Development, 2016
Three studies explored the abilities of 205 children (5-11 years) and 74 adults (18-72 years) to distinguish directly versus indirectly acquired information in a scenario where an individual grew up in isolation from human culture. Directly acquired information is knowledge acquired through firsthand experience. Indirectly acquired information is…
Descriptors: Epistemology, Children, Adults, Older Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Flynn, Emma; Whiten, Andrew – Child Development, 2012
In one of the first open diffusion experiments with young children, a tool-use task that afforded multiple methods to extract an enclosed reward and a child model habitually using one of these methods were introduced into different playgroups. Eighty-eight children, ranging in age from 2 years 8 months to 4 years 5 months, participated. Measures…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Socialization, Young Children, Verbal Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Zelazo, Philip David; Shultz, Thomas R. – Child Development, 1989
Used physical systems with effects of continuous magnitude to examine development of causal prediction in 30 children of 5 and 9 years and adults. There were clear age differences in the ability to integrate information about potency and resistance into sophisticated causal predictions of the magnitude of an effect. (RJC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Developmental Stages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Glidden, Laraine Masters – Child Development, 1977
A multitrial free recall study assessed whether learning-to-learn and changes in strategy over sessions occurred with children in kindergarten and grade 3. Results showed that grade 3 subjects recalled more than did kindergarten subjects, but no learning-to-learn effect was obtained for either age group. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Learning Processes, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Clark, Eve V. – Child Development, 1980
Examines the strategies young children rely on prior to learning how to assign the terms top, bottom, front, and back, and the stages they go through as they master these terms. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Form Classes (Languages), Language Acquisition, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Scholnick, Ellin Kofsky – Child Development, 1971
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Cues, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Birch, Susan A. J.; Bloom, Paul – Child Development, 2002
Two experiments examined young children's use of the familiarity principle when learning language. Found that even 2-year-olds successfully identified the referent of a proper name as the individual with whom the speaker was familiar. However, only 5-year-olds reliably succeeded at determining the individual with whom the speaker was familiar…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Language, Knowledge Level, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McGhee, Paul E. – Child Development, 1974
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Elementary School Students, Humor
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Vinter, Annie; Perruchet, Pierre – Child Development, 2000
Examined implicit learning in 432 four- to 10-year-olds in 3 experiments, using a new paradigm based on drawing behavior. Found that children modified drawing behavior following specially devised practice in such a way that the changes could not be viewed as resulting from deliberate adaptive strategies, with modifications lasting for at least 1…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Freehand Drawing, Learning Processes
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3