NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 27 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ensor, Rosie; Devine, Rory T.; Marks, Alex; Hughes, Claire – Child Development, 2014
Mothers' mental-state references predict individual differences in preschoolers' false-belief (FB) understanding; less is known about the origins of corresponding variation in school-age children. To address this gap, 105 children completed observations with their mothers at child ages 2 and 6, three FB tasks and a verbal comprehension…
Descriptors: Mothers, Theory of Mind, Predictor Variables, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Adamson, Lauren B.; Bakeman, Roger; Deckner, Deborah F.; Nelson, P. Brooke – Child Development, 2014
This research traces the development of symbol-infused joint engagement during mother-child interactions into the preschool years. Forty-nine children, who had been previously observed as toddlers (L. B. Adamson, R. Bakeman, & D. F. Deckner), [Adamson, L. B., 2004], were systematically observed during interactions with their mothers at ages…
Descriptors: Young Children, Parent Child Relationship, Mothers, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roben, Caroline K. P.; Cole, Pamela M.; Armstrong, Laura Marie – Child Development, 2013
Researchers have suggested that as children's language skill develops in early childhood, it comes to help children regulate their emotions (Cole, Armstrong, & Pemberton, 2010; Kopp, 1989), but the pathways by which this occurs have not been studied empirically. In a longitudinal study of 120 children from 18 to 48 months of age, associations…
Descriptors: Language Skills, Toddlers, Psychological Patterns, Self Control
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ganea, Patricia A.; Harris, Paul L. – Child Development, 2010
This research examined the ability of young (N = 96) children to learn about a change in the location of a hidden object, either via an adult's verbal testimony or from direct observation. Thirty-month-olds searched with equal accuracy whether they were told about the change or directly observed it. By contrast, when 23-month-olds were told about…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Interference (Language), Cognitive Development, Deafness
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gauvain, Mary; Munroe, Robert L. – Child Development, 2009
This study examined how societal changes associated with modernization are related to cognitive development. Data were from 4 cultural communities that represented a broad range of traditional and modern elements: the Garifuna (Belize), Logoli (Kenya), Newars (Nepal), and Samoans (American Samoa). Naturalistic observations and the performances of…
Descriptors: Play, Samoan Americans, Foreign Countries, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Masnick, Amy M.; Morris, Bradley J. – Child Development, 2008
A crucial skill in scientific and everyday reasoning is the ability to interpret data. The present study examined how data features influence data interpretation. In Experiment 1, one hundred and thirty-three 9-year-olds, 12-year-olds, and college students (mean age = 20 years) were shown a series of data sets that varied in the number of…
Descriptors: Data Interpretation, Data Analysis, Children, Preadolescents
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ginsburg, Harvey J.; Miller, Shirley M. – Child Development, 1982
Sex differences in risk-taking were examined by observing 480 three- to 11-year-old children at four different risk-taking locations at the San Antonio zoo. While girls were just as likely as boys to enter the zoo, at all four of the risk-taking situations, significantly more boys than girls engaged in risk-taking behavior. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Naturalism, Observation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Butler, Ruth – Child Development, 1989
Gauged the impact of mastery and relative-ability appraisal goals on observational behaviors of 230 children of 4-10 years. Results confirmed that younger children observe others in order to develop mastery, but older children observe to assess relative ability. (RJC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Mastery Learning, Observation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Grath, Gerald; Landers, William F. – Child Development, 1971
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Brody, Gene H.; And Others – Child Development, 1982
Twenty-two subjects (school-age children, their younger siblings, and their best friends) were observed in their homes while playing a popular board game. Five roles were operationalized and observed: teacher, learner, manager, managee, and playmate. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Observation, Peer Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Watson, Malcolm W.; Jackowitz, Elaine R. – Child Development, 1984
Investigates the developmental sequence of learning to transform objects into agents and recipients of action in early symbolic play. Each of 48 children (from 14 to 25 months old) demonstrated initiative pretending after an adult modeled agent and recipient substitutions in pretending to talk on the telephone. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Development, Imitation, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Abramovitch, Rona; Grusec, Joan E. – Child Development, 1978
Five groups of children, ranging in age from 4 to 11 years, were observed for instances of immediate imitation of peers in a free-play setting. Results showed that imitation decreased with age, that more verbal than motor acts were imitated, and that dominant children were imitated more often. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Elementary School Students, Imitation, Observation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Jacobson, Joseph L. – Child Development, 1981
Infants were observed at 10, 12, and 14 1/2 months to test whether social interaction among infant peers develops as a by-product of object-centered play. Age differences were noted. Object-centered contact did not influence early social interaction; long interactions emerged at later ages regardless of whether or not toys were present. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Interaction, Longitudinal Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bar-Tal, Daniel; And Others – Child Development, 1982
One hundred and fifty-six children between the ages of 18 and 76 months were observed three times for 10 minutes each during free play in an attempt to describe the helping behavior of preschool children. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Foreign Countries, Helping Relationship, Observation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ungerer, Judy A.; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Observation indicated that children most frequently represent objects by performing actions appropriate to the represented objects. With increasing age, children represent objects without using functional actions and with objects bearing little physical similarity to the represented objects. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Observation, Perceptual Development
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2